Skip to main content

Removal of reproductive suppression reveals latent sex differences in brain steroid hormone receptors in naked mole-rats, Heterocephalus glaber

Abstract

Background

Naked mole-rats are eusocial mammals, living in large colonies with a single breeding female and 1–3 breeding males. Breeders are socially dominant, and only the breeders exhibit traditional sex differences in circulating gonadal steroid hormones and reproductive behaviors. Non-reproductive subordinates also fail to show sex differences in overall body size, external genital morphology, and non-reproductive behaviors. However, subordinates can transition to breeding status if removed from their colony and housed with an opposite-sex conspecific, suggesting the presence of latent sex differences. Here, we assessed the expression of steroid hormone receptor and aromatase messenger RNA (mRNA) in the brains of males and females as they transitioned in social and reproductive status.

Methods

We compared in-colony subordinates to opposite-sex subordinate pairs that were removed from their colony for either 1 day, 1 week, 1 month, or until they became breeders (i.e., produced a litter). Diencephalic tissue was collected and mRNA of androgen receptor (Ar), estrogen receptor alpha (Esr1), progesterone receptor (Pgr), and aromatase (Cyp19a1) was measured using qPCR. Testosterone, 17β-estradiol, and progesterone from serum were also measured.

Results

As early as 1 week post-removal, males exhibited increased diencephalic Ar mRNA and circulating testosterone, whereas females had increased Cyp19a1 mRNA in the diencephalon. At 1 month post-removal, females exhibited increased 17β-estradiol and progesterone. The largest changes in steroid hormone receptors were observed in breeders. Breeding females had a threefold increase in Cyp19a1 and fivefold increases in Esr1 and Pgr, whereas breeding males had reduced Pgr and increased Ar.

Conclusions

These data demonstrate that sex differences in circulating gonadal steroids and hypothalamic gene expression emerge weeks to months after subordinate animals are removed from reproductive suppression in their home colony.

Background

Sexual differentiation of the vertebrate brain and behavior is mediated in large part by gonadal steroid hormones acting in early development. In mammals, sexually differentiated hormone production begins at the earliest stages of gonadal development and produces marked sexual dimorphism in brain and genitals by early post-natal life (for review, see [1, 2]).

Naked mole-rats (Heterocephalus glaber) do not appear to fit this framework of sexual differentiation: most individuals fail to show traditional sex differences in neuroanatomy or behavior, although these features vary based on reproductive and social status [3–6]. Naked mole-rats are eusocial rodents, living in large colonies with a strict reproductive and social hierarchy: a single female and 1–3 males reproduce and are socially dominant, while others in the colony are subordinate and usually remain reproductively suppressed for the duration of their life [7–9]. The social status within a colony is remarkably stable; however, subordinates can achieve breeder status after loss of a breeder due to death or removal from the colony. In their natural environment, subordinate naked mole-rats can—though rarely—leave their natal colony to find an opposite sex partner and establish their own colony [10, 11]. In captivity, subordinates only undergo sexual maturation if they are separated from their colony [5, 12, 13] or if the established breeders die or are removed [14, 15].

Regardless of the specific circumstances in which subordinates become breeders, they only perform sexual behaviors and show mammalian typical sex differences in gonadal steroid hormones as they transition to breeding status. Subordinate females have lower urinary and plasma progesterone (P) levels as well as plasma luteinizing hormone (LH) concentrations compared to breeder females [16–18]. Within 3 days of separation from their natal colony, subordinate females show increases in urinary P levels, and within 7 days, they develop a perforated vagina [16]. Similarly, male subordinates have lower urinary testosterone (T) compared to male breeders in the colony [14, 15, 19], and urinary T levels and plasma LH increase when subordinate males are removed from their colony [20].

Subordinates and breeders also differ in the neural structures associated with sexual behaviors. However, where sex differences in the nervous system are found in most mammals, status differences are more prominent in the naked mole-rat. For example, breeders, regardless of sex, have larger regional volume in various reproductively relevant hypothalamic structures [4], as well as increased motoneuron soma size in Onuf’s nucleus [3]. Furthermore, androgen receptor (AR) and oxytocin immunoreactivity are influenced by social status more than sex [6, 21, 22].

Collectively, these data reveal a complex interplay between sex and social status in neuroendocrine function in these highly social mammals. To investigate how an apparent reduction or lack of neural sex differences can be associated with typical sex differences in gonadal function seen in breeders, the present study evaluated the time course of changes in gene expression of gonadal steroid hormone receptors and aromatase in brain, as well as associated circulating hormones. Thus, we quantified diencephalic levels of messenger RNA (mRNA) expression of androgen receptor (Ar), estrogen receptor alpha (Esr1), progesterone receptor (Pgr), and aromatase (Cyp19a1) and measured circulating T, 17β-estradiol (E2), and P, as animals transitioned from subordinate to breeding status. We hypothesized that sex differences in steroid hormone receptors would be associated with the differences in circulating gonadal steroids that emerge in breeders, specifically predicting that females would show increases in Esr1 and Pgr while males would show increases in Ar.

Methods

Animals and housing

Sixty-two animals from 11 colonies were used in this experiment. Opposite-sex pairs of animals were established either 1 day (24 h), 1 week (7 days), or 1 month (30 days) prior to tissue collection (6 pairs per time point). Breeders were animals that were paired until a litter was produced (n = 5 pairs) and were collected 30 days after birth of their first litter. In naked mole-rats, gestation is between 65 and 74 days long, and lactation ceases at approximately 28 days post-partum [5, 23]. Taken with the fact that none of the breeding females were pregnant at the time of tissue collection, it seems unlikely that their values reported below were affected by either pregnancy or lactation. Some animals were mis-sexed, resulting in same-sex pairs. Same-sex pairs were excluded from analyses; therefore, the final numbers were five opposite-sex pairs in the 1-day group, 1-week group, and breeder group and six opposite-sex pairs in the 1-month group. In addition, seven male and five female subordinate animals were selected from the colonies immediately prior to tissue collection.

The colonies were housed in polycarbonate cages connected by tubing, as previously described [24]. The paired animals were housed in single polycarbonate cages (L 43 × W 22 × H 21 cm). All animals were kept on a 12:12-light/dark cycle at 28–30 °C and had ad libitum access to a diet of sweet potato and wet protein mash (Harlan Laboratories, Inc). All procedures were approved by the University Animal Care Committee and adhered to institutional and federal guidelines.

Tissue collection

The animals were weighed prior to being overdosed with Avertin (40 mg/100 g). The bloods were collected from the trunk, and the brains were extracted. The brains were bisected at midline in the sagittal plane, frozen in liquid nitrogen, and stored at −80 °C until RNA extraction. The blood samples were kept on wet ice until centrifuged, after which serum was collected and stored at −20 °C.

mRNA expression

The complete diencephalon from the right hemisphere of each subject was dissected and transferred to a sterile tube; every attempt was made to collect the same amount of tissue for all animals. RNA extraction from tissues and complementary DNA (cDNA) was prepared as previously described [25]. For primer sequences, refer to Table 1. Each experimental group (1-day, 1-week, 1-month, and breeder) was run separately with the same-sex subordinates included as controls in each reaction. For each reaction, the expression of the test gene was first normalized to the expression of GAPDH and fold changes were calculated relative to a control group (i.e., same-sex subordinates) as previously described [25].

Table 1 Primer sequences

Hormone assays

Total T was measured from serum using an enzyme-linked immunosorbent assay (ELISA) from Enzo Life Sciences (Cat. No. ADI-901-065), following the manufacturer’s guidelines. The serum was diluted 5× with an assay buffer. The assay sensitivity is 5.67 pg/mL, and the intra-assay coefficient of variation is less than 10 %. Cross-reactivity is 14.6 % for 19-hydroxytestosterone and 7.2 % for androstenedione but less than 1 % for any other hormone or metabolite.

P was measured using an ELISA kit from Cayman Chemical (Cat. No. 582601) according to the manufacturer’s protocol. The serum was diluted 10× with an assay buffer provided. The assay sensitivity is 10 pg/mL, and the intra-assay coefficient of variance is less than 12 %. Cross-reactivity is reported with 17β-estradiol (7.2 %), 5β-pregnan-3α-ol-20-one (6.7 %), pregnenolone (2.5 %), and less than 0.5 % with any other hormone or metabolite.

E2 was measured with an ELISA, following the manufacturer’s protocol (GenWay Biotech, Cat. No. GWB-37E590). The serum was diluted 10× with an assay buffer provided. The assay sensitivity is 3.94 pg/mL, and intra-assay coefficient of variation is less than 10 %. Cross-reactivity is reported with estrone-3-sulfate (4.9 %).

Absorbance for each ELISA plate was measured using a Synergy-HT-Bio-Tek plate reader and was read at 405 nm for T and P and 450 nm for E2, as per the manufacturer’s protocol. All samples were run in duplicate.

Statistical analyses

For mRNA comparisons, we first compared the subordinate values across reactions and evaluated sex differences using a repeated measures ANOVA. We did not find any significant differences between the reactions nor between the sexes (see below), so we then calculated a mean value for each subordinate, for each gene, based on the different reactions. Next, in order to compare the fold change from the subordinate controls, we divided the mRNA value for each experimental subject by the same-sex subordinate group average for each gene. We then conducted a two-way group by sex ANOVA for each target gene. Two-way group by sex ANOVAs were also conducted on the measures of body weight, age, and hormone concentrations. For hormone concentrations, the averages of duplicate readings were corrected by the dilution factor prior to analysis. Tukey’s HSD was used for all post hoc analyses. SPSS version 21 was used for all statistical analyses, and significance was determined by the critical alpha level of <0.05.

Results

mRNA expression

When comparing subordinate values across the different reactions, there were no significant effects of sex or reaction on any gene examined. (Ar: reaction by sex interaction, F (3, 24) = 0.101, p = .959; main effect [ME] of reaction, F (3, 24) = 0.130, p = .942; and ME of sex, F (1, 8) = 0.007, p = .937. Cyp19a1: reaction by sex interaction, F (3, 24) = 0.089, p = .966; ME of reaction, F (3, 24) = 0.063, p = .979; and ME of sex, F (1, 8) = 0.117, p = .741. Esr1: reaction by sex interaction, F (3, 24) = 0.094, p = .962; ME of reaction, F (3, 24) = 0.042, p = .988; and ME of sex, F (1, 8) = 0.046, p = .836. Pgr: reaction by sex interaction, F (3, 24) = 0.053, p = .983; ME of reaction, F (3, 24) = 0.119, p = .948; and ME of sex, F (1, 8) = 0.055, p = .820). For further analyses, we therefore averaged subordinate mRNA values from all reactions and compared them to the other four groups (1-day, 1-week, 1-month, and breeder).

A significant interaction of group and sex was found for Ar, F (4, 43) = 6.398, p < .001, such that male breeders had significantly higher Ar mRNA levels than the subordinates, p = .002, 1-day males, p = .001, and 1-month males, p = .001, although they did not significantly differ from the 1-week males, p = .354. Conversely, the female groups did not differ in Ar mRNA expression (see Fig. 1). A significant main effect of sex, F (1, 43) = 16.76, p < .001, indicates that overall males have more Ar mRNA expression than females. Together, the results indicate changes in Ar mRNA in males, such that AR spikes at 1 week, followed by a decline at 1 month, and then again increases once breeder status is achieved.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Mean fold change of diencephalic mRNA expression compared to subordinate animals ± standard error of the mean. Ar mRNA was significantly higher in male breeders compared to subordinate, 1-day, and 1-month, but not from 1-week, paired males. Females did not show changes in Ar mRNA across conditions, although Cyp19a1 (aromatase) and Esr1 mRNA were both increased in female breeders compared to 1-month, 1-week, 1-day, and subordinate females. Males did not show changes in Esr1 or Cyp19a1 mRNA. Pgr mRNA was decreased in male breeders compared to subordinates and 1-day paired males but did not differ from 1-week and 1-month paired males. Female breeders had significantly increased Pgr mRNA compared to all other female groups. *p < .05. ns non-significant

A significant interaction of group and sex was also found for Cyp19a1, F (4, 43) = 8.712, p < .001; however, in this case, the male groups did not significantly differ from each other (all p > .05). The female breeders had significantly more Cyp19a1 mRNA compared to the 1-month females, p < .001, 1-day females, p < .001, and subordinates, p < .001, and was marginally increased compared to the 1-week females, p = .058. A main effect of sex indicates that females had more Cyp19a1 mRNA compared to males, F (1, 43) = 23.428, p < .001, and the main effect of group indicates that overall breeders had significantly more Cyp19a1 mRNA compared to all other groups (see Fig. 1).

Similar to Cyp19a1, a group by sex interaction, F (4, 43) = 14.131, p < .001, indicates that female breeders have significantly more Esr1 mRNA than all other female groups (1-month, 1-day, 1- week, and subordinate, all p < .001), in addition to a main effect of sex in which females have more Esr1 mRNA than males, F (2, 43) = 20.51, p < .001. The male groups did not significantly differ from each other (see Fig. 1). In our sample, Esr1 mRNA did not show any changes in females until after breeder status was achieved.

Pgr comparisons also revealed a significant group by sex interaction, F (4, 43) = 11.103, p < .001. The male breeders had significantly less Pgr mRNA compared to the subordinates, p = .015, and 1-day males, p = .026, but did not differ from the 1-month, p = .343, nor from the 1-week males, p = .829. Conversely, the female breeders had significantly more Pgr mRNA compared to all female groups (1-month, 1-day, 1-week, and subordinate, p < .01). The 1-month females did not differ from any group other than the female breeders, suggesting that Pgr does not increase in females until a litter is produced (see Fig. 1).

Hormone assays

T concentrations varied according to sex and group, F (4, 45) = 2.664, p = .044. The 1-month males had marginally more T compared to the subordinates, p = .091, whereas females did not show any differences across groups. Although other male groups are not significantly different from each other, there appears to be a trend for males to have an increase in T as early as 1 week after separation from the colony (see Fig. 2). A main effect of sex indicates that males have more T than females, F (1, 45) = 28.526, p < .001, and a main effect of group was trending, F (4, 45) = 2.215, p = .082, indicating a tendency for an increase in T over time away from the colony.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Mean hormone concentration (ng/mL) ± standard error of the mean. An interaction between sex and group indicates that males, but not females, had increased circulating testosterone following 1 week of pairing. Females had increased estradiol and progesterone at 1 month, although estradiol declined after becoming a breeder. Estradiol, but not progesterone, was increased in males that produced a litter. *p < .05; &p < .10. ns non-significant

A sex by group interaction for E2 concentration, F (4, 43) = 6.896, p < .001, indicated that male breeders, but not female breeders, have increased E2 compared to the same-sex subordinate, p < .001; 1-day, p = .002, 1-week, p = .00, and 1-month groups, p < .001. Interestingly, the females showed an increase in E2 at 1 month, compared to the 1-week, p = .009, 1-day, p = .02, and subordinate groups, p = .024, but E2 declined once they produced a litter (see Fig. 2).

P concentrations also varied according to sex and group, F (4, 45) = 5.26, p = .001, such that female breeders, but not males, had increased P compared to the 1-week females, p = .01, 1-day, p = .027, and subordinate females, p = .03, but not the 1-month females, p = .835. There was a trend for the 1-month females to have more P than the 1-week females, suggesting that this hormone increases in females prior to producing a litter. A significant main effect of sex indicated that overall females have more P than males, F (1, 45) = 11.287, p = .002. A main effect of group, F (4, 45) = 4.909, p = .002, suggests that overall breeders have increased P compared to the 1-week, p = .02, 1-day, p = .024, and subordinate groups, p = .024, but not from the 1-month group, p = .967 (see Fig. 2).

Body weight and age

The animals ranged in age from 12 to 38 months of age (M = 23.10, SD = 7.17). No significant differences in age were found between the groups, F (4, 42) = 1.46, p = .231, nor between the sexes, F (1, 42) = 0.631, p = .431. Further, no significant interaction was found between group and sex, F (4, 42) = 0.161, p = .957.

Body weight significantly differed between the groups, F (4, 41) = 5.31, p < .001, but not between sexes, F (1, 41) = 1.41, p = .316 (see Fig. 3), indicating that the breeders weighed more than the subordinates, p < .001; 1-day, p < .001, and 1-week group, p = .001. The 1-month group also significantly differed in weight from the subordinates, p = .009, and 1-week group, p = .008, but did not significantly differ from the 1-day group, p = .104, nor from the breeders, p = .348.

Fig. 3
figure 3

Group means ± standard error of the mean (SEM) for body weight at time of dissections. The breeders weighed significantly more than the animals in the subordinate, 1-day, and 1-week groups. The animals in the 1-month group weighed significantly more than the subordinates and 1-day group, but did not differ from the 1-week group or breeders. Subordinate, 1-day, and 1-week groups did not differ from each other. *p < .05

Discussion

The present study demonstrates that sex-specific changes in neuroendocrine function are associated with the transition to reproductive status in naked mole-rats. We observed the emergence of latent sex differences in diencephalic gene expression and confirmed the emergence of sex differences in circulating gonadal steroids within a week after the animals are removed from reproductive suppression in their home colony. As discussed below, many of these neuroendocrine changes emulate typical sex differences observed in other species during puberty and reproduction, and these results may indicate the endocrine underpinnings of eusociality in naked mole-rats.

The first change we observed in the males was an increase in Ar mRNA expression that mirrored increased circulating T at 1 week post-removal from the colony. The increase in circulating T at this time point is consistent with previous reports in male naked mole-rats [12, 20] and suggests that T action via hypothalamic Ar is associated with the onset of sexual maturation in male naked mole-rats, analogous to other mammalian species. For example, in hamsters, rats, and ferrets, circulating T at puberty is required for male-typical sexual behavior, as well as to suppress female-typical sexual behavior (for review, see [26]). Interestingly, Ar mRNA was not consistently high after removal from the colony. At 1 month, Ar mRNA levels returned to baseline subordinate levels. Transient increases in Ar expression may reflect the termination of an initial phase of reproductive maturation in the breeder males. Alternatively, or additionally, reduced AR protein may be associated with a transition into a parental state. In birds, fish, humans, and other mammals, fathers-to-be exhibit decreases in T prior to the birth of their offspring [27–32], and we have previously reported reduced AR immunoreactivity in the breeding males compared to the subordinates [21, 22]. Decreases in T have been associated with the nurturing aspects of parenting rather than the defense of young/competitive aspects of parenting (for review, see [33]). However, none of our 1-month pairs were obviously pregnant at the time of tissue collection, and T levels remained high in the 1-month paired males. Future experiments evaluating behavior during peaks in Ar mRNA, as well as manipulating circulating T levels, will test both the functional significance of changes in gene expression and the consistency across species of the association between androgens and sexual maturation/mating behavior.

It warrants mention that the status difference in Ar mRNA expression we report here, with the breeding males having a 2.5-fold increase over the subordinate males, is in the opposite direction of status differences in AR protein that we have previously reported [21, 22]. Indeed, increased AR immunoreactivity would be predicted in breeders as androgens typically both stabilize AR protein and facilitate immunohistochemical detection by causing nuclear translocation. Three possibilities might explain this apparent discrepancy. First, our work with AR protein quantified immunoreactivity in particular brain regions whereas the current report does not have the same anatomical resolution. Second, our previous reports compared groups of subordinates and breeders without controlling for duration of pairing, number of litters/size of colony, presence of new litter, etc., whereas the breeders herein were all collected exactly 30 days after the birth of their first litter. Finally, patterns of AR protein and mRNA expression are not always complementary (for review, see [34]). This raises the possibility that, in this case, non-coordinate regulation of AR may be achieved via differences in mRNA translation. Although this is an uncommon mechanism, naked mole-rats have a ribosomal organization which is unique in eukaryotic organisms [35] and could very well have similarly unique regulation of protein translation. It will be important to directly quantify protein and mRNA in the same animals to test these alternatives.

In addition to the increase in Ar mRNA after the production of a litter, male naked mole-rats exhibit other neuroendocrine changes similar to new fathers from other species. For example, circulating E2 increased and Pgr mRNA decreased in new naked mole-rat fathers. In humans, an increase in E2 has been reported in men becoming fathers that lasted from the last month of pregnancy to the first month after pregnancy [28]. Reduced Pgr has also been associated with increased paternal behavior in mice: Pgr-knockout mice and pharmacological antagonism of Pgr increased parental care and decreased infanticide behavior without diminishing male-male aggression, whereas increased P has the opposite effect ([36]; for review, see [37]). Alternatively, it is possible that this small decrease in Pgr is not biologically functional. Future experiments could evaluate whether PR has a role similar to other species in male naked mole-rats.

In female naked mole-rats, the first observed endocrine change was an increase in circulating E2 and P 1 month post-removal from their colony. This increase may be indicative of sexual maturation and sexual receptivity. Estrogens are known to trigger, and P to reinforce, the events leading to ovulation in other rodent species (for review, see [38, 39]). E2 is also related to lordosis behavior in rodents, and P acting via progesterone receptor amplifies this behavior (for review, see [40]). Increases in Esr1 and Pgr mRNA were not apparent in female naked mole-rats until after the production of a litter; thus, it is possible that increases in circulating E2 and P are responsible for the increase in diencephalic mRNA expression of the corresponding receptors. However, it is also possible that this increase in gene expression is related to new motherhood, as in other species, increases in Esr1 are also associated with maternal care [41].

The timing of our increase in E2 and P at 1 month post-removal is later then we predicted based on reports of increased P in the urine of female naked mole-rats approximately 1 week after removal from the colony [12, 16]. This might reflect different hormone concentrations in urine versus blood, differences in assay sensitivity, or other methodological differences concerning sample collection. We collected the blood at a single time point whereas previous reports collected urine at multiple times per animal [12, 16]. Alternatively, the variability between experiments might reflect individual differences in the timing of reproductive maturation in subordinate naked mole-rats. New pairs of opposite-sex subordinates show substantial variability in the amount of time to produce their first litter, ranging from 2 months to over 2 years [Holmes, unpublished observations]. The five breeding pairs included here ranged from 3.5 to 6.5 months to produce their first litter. It would be interesting to test whether variability in the onset of endocrine changes corresponds with the variability in the time it takes a new pair of naked mole-rats to produce a litter.

Unexpectedly, we observed a small increase in Cyp19a1 mRNA expression 1 week post-removal in females and a marked increase in females after the production of a litter. These increases were not seen in males and could be related to the dominant status the breeding female undertakes in the colony. Aromatase has been linked to aggression and dominance in other species. For example, Unger et al. [42] report that hypothalamic aromatase regulates aggression in both male and female mice, and Huffman, O’Connell, and Hofmann [43] report that an aromatase inhibitor decreases aggression in cichlid fish [42, 43]. Currently, little is known about how naked mole-rat female breeders maintain their dominance over others in the colony; therefore, it would be of interest to evaluate the role of aromatase given the increase in Cyp19a1 during the transition to breeder status. Another possibility, which is not mutually exclusive, is that increased Cyp19a1 mRNA could be related to female sexual behavior. Observations from Cyp19a1 knockout mice suggest that aromatase may be necessary for female-typical sexual behaviors (for review, see [44]). Similarly in prairie voles, aromatase inhibitors reduce female sexual behaviors [45]. Regardless, dominance and sexual behaviors are both important for a naked mole-rat female to establish and maintain her position at the top of the social hierarchy. Evaluating the role that aromatase plays in social status and reproduction in naked mole-rats will shed light on key features of eusociality in mammals, including how breeding females suppress reproductive maturation in their colony mates. Given that aromatase converts testosterone to estradiol and we report low levels of circulating testosterone in all female groups, the increased Cyp19a1 mRNA in breeding females could be associated with aromatase acting on other androgenic substrates. Alternatively, our single sampling paradigm might have missed cyclic variation in circulating androgens or reflect a dissociation between androgens in the blood and brain.

Finally, the group differences we see in body weight are consistent with previous reports that breeders, particularly queens, are often the largest animals in the colony (e.g., [9]). Furthermore, queen removal results in increased body weight of both male and female subordinates [14], suggesting the release from reproductive suppression triggers growth in non-reproductive animals. Indeed, new queens demonstrate remarkable vertebral lengthening associated with parity [46], which might also be associated with the release from reproductive suppression and representative of a pubertal growth spurt [47].

Conclusions

The present study indicates that naked mole-rats exhibit sex-specific neuroendocrine changes when transitioning from subordination to reproductive maturation and social dominance. Naked mole-rats transitioning to breeding status resemble other mammalian species during puberty and begin to exhibit endocrine changes as early as 1 week following removal from the reproductive suppression of their colony. The sex-specific activation of gonadal steroid hormones and their receptors in the brain are clearly directly linked to reproduction. However, some of the neuroendocrine changes we report might also reflect sex-specific demands in colony living (e.g., dominance in breeding females and paternal behavior in breeding males). Given that reproductive hierarchies and large social colonies are two defining features of eusociality, these data reveal much about how this extreme form of social organization occurs in mammals. In conclusion, these data (a) confirm that naked mole-rats show sex differences in circulating gonadal hormones only when released from reproductive suppression, (b) demonstrate that latent sex differences in gonadal steroid hormone receptors exist in the brain of these remarkably monomorphic animals, and (c) provide future directions for evaluating the neuroendocrine basis of eusociality in naked mole-rats.

Abbreviations

Ar :

androgen receptor gene

AR:

androgen receptor protein

Cyp19a1 :

aromatase gene

E2:

17β-estradiol

ELISA:

enzyme-linked immunosorbent assay

Esr1 :

estrogen receptor alpha gene

LH:

luteinizing hormone

P:

progesterone

Pgr :

progesterone receptor gene

T:

testosterone

References

  1. Cooke B, Hegstrom CD, Villeneuve LS, Breedlove SM. Sexual differentiation of the vertebrate brain: principles and mechanisms. Front Neuroendocrinol. 1998;19(4):323–62.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Arnold A. The organizational-activational hypothesis as the foundation for a unified theory of sexual differentiation of all mammalian tissues. Horm Behav. 2009;55:570–8.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Seney M, Goldman BD, Forger NG. Breeding status affects motoneurons and muscle size in naked mole-rats: recruitment of perineal motoneurons? J Neurobiol. 2006;66(12):1354–64.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Holmes MM, Rosen GJ, Jordan CL, de Vries GJ, Goldman BD, Forger NG. Social control of brain morphology in a eusocial mammal. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2007;104:10548–52.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Holmes MM, Goldman BD, Goldman SL, Seney ML, Forger NG. Neuroendocrinology and sexual differentiation in eusocial mammals. Front Neuroendocrinol. 2009;30:519–33.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Mooney SJ, Holmes MM. Social condition and oxytocin neuron number in the hypothalamus of naked mole-rats (Heterocephalus glaber). Neurosci. 2013;230:56–61.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  7. Jarvis JUM. Eusociality in a mammal: cooperative breeding in naked mole-rat colonies. Science. 1981;212:571–3.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Brett RA. The population structure of naked mole-rat colonies. In: Sherman PW, Jarvis JUM, Alexander RD, editors. The biology of the naked mole-rat. USA: Princeton University Press; 1991. p. 97–136.

    Google Scholar 

  9. Lacey EA, Sherman PW. Social organization of naked mole-rat colonies: evidence for divisions of labor. In: Sherman PW, Jarvis JUM, Alexander RD, editors. The biology of the naked mole-rat. USA: Princeton University Press; 1991. p. 275–336.

    Google Scholar 

  10. Braude S. Dispersal and new colony formation in wild naked mole-rats: evidence against inbreeding as the system of mating. Behav Ecol. 2000;11:7–12.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. O’Riain MJ, Jarvis JUM, Faulkes CG. A dispersive morph in the naked mole-rat. Nature. 1996;380:619–21.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Faulkes CG, Abbott DH. Evidence that primer pheromones do not cause social suppression of reproduction in male and female naked mole-rats (Heterocephalus glaber). J Reprod Fertil. 1993;99:225–30.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Smith TE, Faulkes CG, Abbott DH. Combined olfactory contact with parent colony and direct contact with nonbreeding animals does not maintain suppression of ovulation in naked mole-rats (Heterocephalus glaber). Horm Behav. 1997;31(3):277–88.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Clarke FM, Faulkes CG. Dominance and queen succession in captive colonies of the eusocial naked mole-rat, Heterocephalus glaber. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci. 1997;264:993–1000.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  15. Clarke FM, Faulkes CG. Hormonal and behavioural correlates of male dominance and reproductive status in captive colonies of the naked mole-rat, Heterocephalus glaber. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci. 1998;265:1391–9.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  16. Faulkes CG, Abbott DH, Jarvis JU. Social suppression of ovarian cyclicity in captive and wild colonies of naked mole-rats, Heterocephalus glaber. J Reprod Fertil. 1990;88:559–68.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Faulkes CG, Abbott DH, Liddell CE, George LM, Jarvis JUM. Hormonal and behavioral aspects of reproductive suppression in female naked mole-rats, Heterocephalus glaber. In: Sherman PW, Jarvis JUM, Alexander RD, editors. The biology of the naked mole-rat. USA: Princeton University Press; 1991. p. 426–45.

    Google Scholar 

  18. Margulis SW, Saltzman W, Abbott DH. Behavioral and hormonal changes in female naked mole-rats (Heterocephalus glaber) following removal of the breeding female from a colony. Horm Behav. 1995;29:227–47.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Faulkes CG, Abbott DH, Jarvis JUM. Social suppression of reproduction in male naked mole-rats, Heterocephalus glaber. J Reprod Fertil. 1991;91:593–604.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Faulkes CG, Abbott DH. Social control of reproduction in breeding and non-breeding male naked mole-rats (Heterocephalus glaber). J Reprod Fertil. 1991;93(2):427–35.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Holmes MM, Goldman BD, Forger NG. Social status and sex independently influence androgen receptor expression in the eusocial naked mole-rat brain. Horm Behav. 2008;54:278–85.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Holmes MM, Van Mil S, Bulkowski C, Goldman BD, Forger NG. Androgen receptor distribution in the social decision-making network of eusocial naked mole-rats. Behav Brain Res. 2013;256:214–8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Dengler-Crish CM, Catania KC. Cessation of reproduction-related spine elongation after multiple breeding cycles in female naked mole rats. Anat Rec. 2009;292(1):131–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Mooney SJ, Douglas NR, Holmes MM. Peripheral administration of oxytocin increases social affiliation in the naked mole-rat (Heterocephalus glaber). Horm Behav. 2014;65(4):380–5.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Monks DA, Johansen JA, Mo K, Rao P, Eagleson B, Yu Z, et al. Overexpression of wild-type androgen receptor in muscle recapitulates polyglutamine disease. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2007;104(46):18259–64.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Schulz KM, Sisk CL. Pubertal hormones, the adolescent brain, and the maturation of social behaviors: lessons from the Syrian hamster. Mol Cel Endocrin. 2006;154:120–6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Archer J. Testosterone and human aggression: an evaluation of the challenge hypothesis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2006;30:319–45.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Berg SJ, Wynne-Edwards KE. Changes in testosterone, cortisol, and estradiol levels in men becoming fathers. Mayo Clin Proc. 2001;76(6):582–92.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Goymann W. Social modulation of androgens in male birds. Gen Comp Endocrinol. 2009;163:149–57.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Hirschenhauser K, Oliveira R. Social modulation of androgens in male vertebrates: meta-analyses of the challenge hypothesis. Anim Behav. 2006;71:265–77.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  31. Kou R, Chou SY, Chen SC, Huang ZY. Juvenile hormone and the ontogeny of cockroach aggression. Horm Behav. 2009;56:332–8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Muller MN, Wrangham RW. Dominance, aggression and testosterone in wild chimpanzees: a test of the ‘challenge hypothesis’. Anim Behav. 2004;67:113–23.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. van Anders SM. Beyond masculinity: testosterone, gender/sex, and human social behavior in a comparative context. Front Neuroendocrinol. 2013;34(3):198–210.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Maier T, Guell M, Serrano L. Correlation of mRNA and protein in complex biological samples. FEBS Lett. 2009;583:3966–73.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Azpurua J, Ke Z, Chen IX, Zhang Q, Ermolenko DN, Zhang ZD, et al. Naked mole-rat has increased translational fidelity compared with the mouse, as well as a unique 28S ribosomal RNA cleavage. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2013;110:17350–5.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Schneider JS, Stone MK, Wynne-Edwards KE, Horton TH, Lydon JP, O’Malley B, et al. Progesterone receptors mediate male aggression towards infants. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2003;100:2951–6.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Wynne-Edwards KE, Timonin ME. Paternal care in rodents: weakening support for hormonal regulation of the transition to behavioral fatherhood in rodent animal models of biparental care. Horm Behav. 2007;52:114–21.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Fink G. Oestrogen and progesterone interactions in the control of gonadotrophin and prolactin secretion. J Steroid Biochem. 1988;30:169–78.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Clarkson J, Herbison AE. Oestrogen, kisspeptin, GPR54 and the pre-ovulory luteinizing hormone surge. J Neuroendocrinol. 2009;21(4):305–11.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Mong JA, Pfaff DW. Hormonal and genetic influences underlying arousal as it drives sex and aggression in animal and human brains. Neurobiol Aging. 2003;24:S83–8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Champagne FA, Weaver ICG, Diorio J, Sharma S, Meaney MJ. Natural variations in maternal care are associated with estrogen receptor alpha expression and estrogen sensitivity in the medial preoptic area. Endocrinol. 2003;144:4720–4.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  42. Unger EK, Burke Jr KJ, Yang CF, Bender KJ, Fuller PM, Shah NM. Medial amygdalar aromatase neurons regulate aggression in both sexes. Cell Rep. 2015;10(4):453–62.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Huffman LS, O’Connell LA, Hofmann HA. Aromatase regulates aggression in the African cichlid fish Astatotilapia burtoni. Physiol Behav. 2013;112–113:77–83.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Bakker J, Honda S, Harada N, Balthazart J. The aromatase knockout mouse provides new evidence that estrogens are required for the development of the female brain. Ann NY Acad Sci. 2003;1007:251–62.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Northcutt KV, Lonstein JS. Sex differences and effects of neonatal aromatase inhibition on masculine and feminine copulatory potentials in prairie voles. Horm Behav. 2008;54:160–9.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Henry EC, Dengler-Crish CM, Catania KC. Growing out of a caste: reproduction and the making of the queen mole-rat. J Exp Biol. 2007;210:261–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Dengler-Crish CM, Catania KC. Phenotypic plasticity in female naked mole-rats after removal from reproductive suppression. J Exp Biol. 2007;210:4351–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The research reported in this manuscript was supported by Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada (NSERC) Discovery Grants 312458 (DAM) and 402633 (MMH), a Connaught Foundation New Investigator award to MMH, a doctoral Canadian graduate scholarship (CGS) NSERC grant to ASG, and a doctoral Postgraduate scholarship (PGS) NSERC to DEP.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Melissa M. Holmes.

Additional information

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Authors’ contributions

ASG, DAM, and MMH were responsible for the conception and design of the study. ASG and DP performed the manipulations and sample collection. KM and ASG performed the qPCR. ASG and DP performed the ELISA. ASG, KM, DP, and MMH performed the statistical analyses. ASG, MMH, and DAM wrote the manuscript. All authors revised the manuscript and have given the final approval.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Swift-Gallant, A., Mo, K., Peragine, D.E. et al. Removal of reproductive suppression reveals latent sex differences in brain steroid hormone receptors in naked mole-rats, Heterocephalus glaber . Biol Sex Differ 6, 31 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1186/s13293-015-0050-x

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s13293-015-0050-x

Keywords