Skip to main content

Considering sex and gender in Epidemiology: a challenge beyond terminology. From conceptual analysis to methodological strategies

Abstract

Background

Epidemiologists need tools to measure effects of gender, a complex concept originating in the social sciences which is not easily operationalized in the discipline. Our aim is to clarify useful concepts, measures, paths, effects, and analytical strategies to explore mechanisms of health difference between men and women.

Methods

We reviewed concepts to clarify their definitions and limitations for their translation into usable measures in Epidemiology. Then we conducted methodological research using a causal framework to propose methodologically appropriate strategies for measuring sex and gender effects in health.

Results

(1) Concepts and measures. We define gender as a set of norms prescribed to individuals according to their attributed-at-birth sex. Gender pressure creates a systemic gap, at population level, in behaviors, activities, experiences, etc., between men and women. A pragmatic individual measure of gender would correspond to the level at which an individual complies with a set of elements constituting femininity or masculinity in a given population, place and time. (2) Main analytical strategy. Defining and measuring gender are not sufficient to distinguish the effects of sex and gender on a health outcome. We should also think in terms of mechanisms, i.e., how the variables are linked together, to define appropriate analytical strategies. A causal framework can help us to conceptualize “sex” as a “parent” of a gender or gendered variable. This implies that we cannot interpret sex effects as sexed mechanisms, and that we can explore gendered mechanisms of sex-differences by mediation analyses. (3) Alternative strategy. Gender could also be directly examined as a mechanism, rather than through a variable representing its realization in the individual, by approaching it as an interaction between sex and social environment.

Conclusions

Both analytical strategies have limitations relative to the impossibility of reducing a complex concept to a single or a few measures, and of capturing the entire effect of the phenomenon of gender. However, these strategies could lead to more accurate analyses of the mechanisms underlying health differences between men and women.

Highlights

  • A pragmatic individual measure of individual gender would correspond to the level at which an individual complies with a set of elements constituting femininity or masculinity in a given population, place and time

  • Comparing outcomes by sex and gender is not sufficient, and even misleading, to understand the mechanisms underlying biological and health differences between men and women

  • Causal analysis framework is a powerful tool for refining hypotheses and identifying the effects that can and should be estimated to meet the objectives

  • Gender could also be measured at the populational-level as an interaction between sex and environment, which would be more compatible with the sociological concept of gender

Introduction

For more than a decade, scholars have been working to clarify sex and gender terms for biomedical research and have emphasized that they should not be confused [1,2,3,4,5]. However, although the importance of taking these factors into account in epidemiologyFootnote 1 has been repeatedly emphasized [6, 7], their true integration into practice remains marginal and often approximate [8]. This observation could be due to a poor understanding of the definitions by health researchers, since these terms are often used as synonyms, or interchangeably in scientific papers [3, 9,10,11,12]. We hypothesize that these persistent difficulties are partly based on lack of adaptation of these concepts imported from disciplines that do not have same challenges, paradigms and methods as epidemiology. We aim to transpose considerations about sex and gender developed in social sciences and humanities and propose a pragmatic operationalization of them, so that they can be better implemented in our field.

In the medical sciences, definitions of the terms "sex" and "gender" consensually follow the classic definitions given by sociologist Oakley in the 1970s [13]: sex refers to biological differences between women and men [1, 2] and gender refers to observed, experienced, prescribed or favored social differences, based on attributed-at-birth sex [1,2,3,4]. The concept of "sex" can be more precisely understood as a social construct [14, 15], premised upon a set of biological characteristics of different natures (genes, hormones, anatomy, etc.), directly or indirectly in connection with reproductive function, and on average strongly correlated with each other within each sex category [2, 10]. About the concept of “gender”, the minimalistic definition of "social differences between men and women" [1] hides the complexity of this concept. Gender is indeed a complex phenomenon which is continuous (femininity ↔ masculinity) but strongly premised upon the binary classification of sex, multidimensional (traits, norms, stereotypes, roles, responsibilities, activities, etc.), multi-level (experienced by individuals and prescribed by society, at different structural levels, and possibly heterogeneously), intersecting (with age, ethnicity, class, etc.), highly contextual, evolving over the life course, and across generations, and highly diffuse (in society, family, work… in relations, in expectations, in perceptions, in actions, etc.).

Epidemiology sometimes investigates differences in health between men and women. As such, sex can be regarded as the exposure of interest. In this case, the central question posed is: why are there differences in health between men and women? The phenomena at stake in these differences can be social and/or biological and therefore involve the concepts of sex and gender in a deeply intertwined way. For example, why are little boys more likely to have allergic diseases than little girls? Are there biological predispositions related to biological sex or is it the socially defined gendered exposures that explains it? Of course, the nature of the mechanisms sometimes seems obvious. Especially if the outcome is social: it seems obvious that the difference in wages between men and women is not directly determined by their respective reproductive physiology. This is less obvious for "biological" differences such as cholesterol levels, cortisol levels, allostatic load, etc. Some researchers implicitly consider that if a variable is biological then its mechanisms are sexed and not gendered (see the “Course 3: Sex and Gender in the Analysis of Secondary Data from Human Participants” of “Canadian Institutes of Health Research” website [16]). Yet social factors, like gender, can be “embodied” during life course [17, 18] and therefore have effects on biological processes. So, it is important to identify what, in biological and health differences between men and women, is gender-related and therefore may be modifiable by implementing prevention measures targeted at the relevant determinants.

To answer these questions, epidemiologists need tools to capture the sex and gender phenomena and these tools must be compatible with the discipline's methods. The first issue might be the need to contain the complex concept of gender in one or several individual variables to be used in epidemiological analyses. But questioning the influence of sex and gender on health does not only require correct measures, but also analytical approaches aiming to capture the phenomena of sex and gender in their complexity and in isolation one from another.

Our aim here was to clarify concepts, measures, paths, effects, and analytical strategies useful to explore mechanisms of health difference between men and women. We first reviewed concepts to clarify their definitions and limitations for their translation into usable measures in Epidemiology. We then conducted methodological research using a causal framework to propose a methodologically appropriate strategy for measuring gender effects in health, based on mediation analysis. Finally, we propose an alternative strategy based on the estimation of the interaction effect between sex and social environment.

Gender, from a population-level concept to an individual variable

How gender can be conceptualized in epidemiology

Gender is a differential social construct: it corresponds to the fact that norms of different kinds (behaviors, activities, experiences) are differently prescribed to individuals according to their attributed-at-birth sex. The gender system divides humanity according to the typology known as "sex" and associates values, objects and properties of different kinds with each of these categories. For example, physical strength, the color blue and mathematics might be considered as masculine objects, in a given place and time. This is consistent with a sociological approach which refers to gender as a process of social division: "gender is the system of hierarchical division of humanity into two unequal halves" [19]. Through socialization, we suggest that the process of division will be realized through individuals by a sex-differentiated normative pressure. This hypothesis follows Durkheim’s definition of social fact: "these types of conduct or thoughts are external to the individual but, they possess an imperative and coercive power by virtue of which they are imposed on them, whether they like it or not" [20]. This normative pressure will, in our example, encourage men to develop their strength, to wear blue clothes, to love mathematics. This differential normative pressure, based on attributed sex, creates a systematic and systemic difference between men and women in society, observed at population level. Indeed, even if individuals do not adopt all valued attributes of their sex, a difference will be observed in the distribution of these attributes among individuals categorized by sex at the population level.

Translation into an individual measure

Gender is socially performed, through a systemic normative pressure, and observed as a distributional gap at the population level. However, in epidemiology, variables are typically defined at the individual level to be linked to other variables like health outcome. We observed three ways of measuring gender in epidemiology: gender identity, gender personality and gender diagnosis.

  1. (a)

    Gender is often associated with gender identity, measured by self-reporting from an individual. This dimension of gender can be defined as how an individual sees themselves on the continuum of socially prescribed femininity or masculinity or outside this continuum [2]. It is highly dependent upon social prescriptions and other social determinants (e.g., capacity for self-determination) and is not necessarily related to what the individual performs through their behaviors, activities, etc.

  2. (b)

    We also find gender personality scores such as: Bem-Sex-role-inventory [21], Conformity to Masculine Norms Inventory [22]. The measurement of these scores needs to be planned beforehand. Moreover, these scores are based on stereotypes and are not sensitive to the context of generation, culture, age, class, race, etc.

  3. (c)

    Measures of "Gender diagnosis" kind [23] are composite individual indicators which can be constructed from data, based on the presence or absence of several gendered dimensions, defined from the sex-differential distribution of these dimensions in the population. This indicator corresponds to a measure of the level at which an individual complies with a set of elements constituting femininity or masculinity in a given population, place and time, i.e., as a kind of probability of being "predicted male" from dimensions associated with masculinity, or "predicted female" from dimensions associated with femininity. This means considering an individual as being more or less masculine because they have a greater or fewer number of masculine characteristics, considered as such because these characteristics are more frequent or of a higher value among men within the population studied. This is a common measurement method of gender in gender-sensitive studies [24, 25].

Strength and limitations of gender-diagnosis measures

“Gender-diagnosis” measures are consistent with the way gender can be conceptualized at the individual level in epidemiology as the result of normative pressure resulting in a differential probability of having this or that characteristic according to birth-sex. This method can be used in secondary data analysis and also takes into account the context by being defined for a given population, so based on the specific norms of that population.

It has some limitations, however, which require precautions. Firstly, we will obviously never be able to capture the totality of such a diffuse and complex phenomenon in one or several variables. Secondly, as this phenomenon is not necessarily consistent at the individual level, we will lose information by reducing it into a score. For example, a man may have a job considered to be feminine, such as caregiving or midwifery, but display characteristics considered to be masculine in his family environment (measured from the burden of domestic tasks for example). To take this into account, several variables related to the different ways of characterizing groups and contexts could also be defined, depending on available data and constraints of the research question, for example "occupational gender" and "domestic gender", or, more often, "gender role" and "gender relationship" [5]. But we inevitably lose some of the nuance.

Thirdly, the presence of one or more gendered dimensions in an individual is not necessarily due to gender pressure alone. For example, if, in a given group, smoking is much more common among men than among women, this behavior will be considered as masculine. But the fact that an individual smokes is not determined solely by this mechanism. It is not only a marker of a person’s gender, but also a marker of other social determinants such as socio-economic position, social network, etc. If we observe an effect of this gender score on an outcome in a sex group, it is therefore difficult to interpret whether this effect is really due to the gender phenomenon we are trying to capture or to other factors determining the score.

We cannot therefore consider a gendered dimension, as a ‘pure and perfect’ proxy of individual gender. This limitation should be kept in mind when using gendered variables as gender markers. Despite its limitations, this “gender diagnosis method” remains a pragmatic tool.

It is not just about sex and gender, it is about mechanisms

Visualize links and context, using directed acyclic graphs

Even if the concepts of sex and gender have been well defined, differentiated and measured, with all the limitations mentioned, this is still not sufficient to isolate and analyze the effects of sex and gender on a health outcome, denoted Y. Mainly because individual gender, defined as the result of gender pressure on an individual, is strongly associated with sex: if a newborn baby is defined as male, he will be socialized as a boy, whereas if defined as female, she will be socialized as a girl. The gendered characteristics that each child will have, even if modulated by other social and individual factors, thus strongly depend on their sex, which is a "parent” (direct cause) of these characteristics, in a causal-framework sense. Therefore, an association between an individual score of gender and an outcome Y cannot be interpreted as proof that gender pressure explains part of Y, because sex is a confounder in this association. The reverse interpretation would be equally flawed: we cannot conclude from an association between birth-sex and an outcome Y that biological mechanisms only explain this association and not the gender pressure, because the effect of sex on Y can be mediated by (= can pass through) gender.

It is therefore insufficient to simply avoid confusing sex with gender concepts and the variables that measure them, we also have to avoid confusing the mechanisms that relate one to the other. To grasp these issues, we propose to use causal-approach tools to clearly identify the mechanisms of interest and, on this basis, define our analysis strategy: directed acyclic graphs (DAG) and counterfactual notations [26, 27]. The principle is to visually represent all the variables of interest (the "nodes"), measured or not, and all the possible causal links between these variables (the "arrows"). This tool allows us (1) to be transparent about the a priori hypotheses regarding the underlying causal structure; (2) to precisely define the effect to be estimated in order to meet the objectives (which can be expressed using counterfactual notations); (3) to build the appropriate model to identify and estimate this effect, taking into account the context and thus avoid the main methodological biases like not adjusting on a confounder or adjusting on a mediator, etc. [27].

Figure 1 is an example of DAG, allowing us to visualize the sequence of causes and therefore the whole causal structure. This graph represents a very general scenario of a sequence of two exposures X1 and X2 that cause an outcome Y, each node representing a variable or set of variables. Fundamental and independent determinants of X1, X2 and Y are innate factors, including sex, and environmental factors.

Fig. 1
figure 1

General graph of causal links

What are "sex effects" and "gender effects"?

Strictly speaking, the “effect of sex” on Y corresponds to all directed paths that begin at the Sex node and end at Y (double arrows in Fig. 2a). However, it is sometimes implicitly suggested that when we talk about the “effect of sex”, we are only talking about biological mechanisms and that we are therefore only referring to paths that would not pass through social factors. In fact, this “biological effect of sex” would be the direct effect (double continuous arrow in Fig. 2b) and the indirect effects which pass through exposures not linked to the environment (double dashed arrows, with the hypothesis of independence between Env and X1), and assuming that no mediators with social–environmental determinant have been omitted.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Effects of sex and gender: Total effect of Sex (a), Biologic effect of Sex (b), Effect of a gendered variable (c) and Effect of a gender variable (d)

When we observe a result where there is an association between Sex and an outcome Y, this finding corresponds to the total effect of Sex on Y (Fig. 2a). Again, we cannot know if this total effect is explained by biological or social mechanisms, even if we have well defined the Sex variable as a biological phenomenon. It is therefore important to determine if this is really the effect of interest. By using these graphic representations, we can also highlight the complexity of isolating the biological effect of sex, which would require us to first make the strong assumption of an independence between the environment and all the intermediate factors (as for X1 in our example), and second to "block" all other paths to identify the direct effect.

We can also focus on gendered exposure(s). For example, if the probability of playing football X is different according to birth-sex and to the place of residence, we will say that this activity is socially determined and gendered. We would want to identify the risk factors for a rupture of the anterior cruciate ligament Y, assuming that there is no direct effect of sex on the probability of this pathology occurring but an effect of playing football X (see Fig. 2c). Since playing football is a risk factor for the disease and a gendered activity, we would find here a “sex effect”, i.e., statistical association and even a causal pathway (mediated by football X) between the variable Sex and Y. In this example, X is a gendered activity, but we could have used another gendered dimension, gender identity, a set of gendered variables or a gender diagnosis, as described above and as represented in Fig. 2d. This figure allows us to visualize the potential confounding effect of sex and environment when we look at the effect of this kind of gender marker on Y. If we wanted to identify and measure the specific effect of individual gender, we would have to make sure that we could control all these confounders. These examples demonstrate that it is necessary to ensure that the assumptions regarding mechanisms and pathways of interest are clearly defined a priori.

Exploring mechanisms with mediation analysis

When we want to understand the health effects of sex and gender, i.e., describe them, distinguish them and explore their mechanisms, different questions can be addressed that do not involve the same analytical strategies. If the question is: “Are the differences in health observed between men and women explained, at least partially, by social mechanisms?”, then our focus will be on the pathways operating through social dimensions of a Sex → Y effect, i.e., in the socially mediated indirect effect of sex. If the question is “Does a gendered dimension(s), like a gender diagnosis, have an effect on health?”, then our focus will be on the total effect of a gendered exposure, as described in Fig. 2c. It is therefore important to distinguish, name, and define the multiple pathways that link sex and gender to the outcome.

Based on the causal framework, mediation analysis strategies [28, 29] have been defined in order to estimate mediated effects. These strategies could allow us to answer questions such as "how much of the sex-difference on that outcome is explained by gendered behaviors?” for example. Based on these methods and on the counterfactual formulation ("if the situation had not been as it is"), we propose a typology of several effects of interest in Table 1, with corresponding examples of counterfactual formulation. We will denote YS=s or YS=s, E=e the potential outcome had a subject been exposed, respectively, to the counterfactual interventions S = s or {S = s and E = e}. In this table, the gender variable is described as a binary variable G = {f;m} in order to simplify the presentation rather than for a conceptual reason.

Table 1 Typology of sex S and gender G effects on a health outcome Y

Ideally in this typology, G should represent "being / acting / living / etc. as a man" (or "as a woman"), i.e., everything that socially makes a man (or a woman) in a given time, place and population. In this case, the direct effect, RES (what does not pass through G), would correspond to the non-socially mediated or the biological effect of sex in these time, place and population. But, as we said before, gender is so diffuse that it is impossible to think that we can capture all its dimensions in one or a few variables. With this analytical strategy, at most we can: (1) verify the hypothesis that social pathways (SMIES) explain, at least in part, a sex effect (TES), and (2) have an order of magnitude of the biological effect of sex on a phenomenon Y depending on the conceptual extensivity of G. But a RES can never be said to be the pure biological effect of sex, even if we have considered many gendered dimensions in G.

An alternative analytical strategy: gender as a sex–environment interaction

Gender as a sex–environment interaction: conceptual arguments

Rather than using individual variables as proxies for a population-level phenomenon, gender could also be directly examined as the mechanism. This mechanism can be considered as an interaction between sex and social environment. Indeed, we said above that a dimension is gendered when it is a descendant of both sex and social environment, but there are cases where a dimension is a descendant of both sex and social environment and is not gendered. For example, let us imagine that the head circumference of newborns is on average different depending on sex at birth. In a given society, pregnant women of one caste eat differently from others and this diet has an effect on the head circumference of newborns. In this society, however, the sex of the child before birth is not known. In this case, the newborn’s cranial perimeter is a descendant of sex and the environment, but it is not gendered. It would be if the sex of their unborn child was known and if the pregnant women also ate differently according to this knowledge. So, the dimension is said to be gendered not only when it differs according to sex and to the social environment, but also when the environmental cause varies according to sex. It is the definition of an interaction phenomenon. This is why we can refer to gender as a "differential distributor" of social exposures. In terms of social and biological explanations, a sex-difference that exists whatever the social group and the culture is likely to be biological, but if its effect varies greatly according to social classes or cultures, it may be mediated by social mechanisms. This echoes anthropologist Margaret Mead’s conclusions that temperaments (gentleness, violence, etc.) attributed to men or women did not stem from biological sex but were socially constructed because they varied from one society to another [30].

Gender as a sex–environment interaction: strategic implications

We can start from this definition to define a strategy that will consider gender as an interaction between sex and the social environment. Counterfactual formulations of interaction effects have also been proposed in the causal framework, as well as a methodology to choose the scale (additive or multiplicative), to estimate these effects and to present results [31]. We denoted the social environment E, with E = 0 the social-environment group of reference (the less gendered group ideally, as a non-gendered environment generally does not exist) and E = 1 the other group. We denoted S the attributed-at-birth sex. We want to distinguish the effect of sex, which would occur in the reference group; the effect of the environment, not related to a sex effect; and the effect of gender, as a sex-differentiated effect of the environment, or a socially varying effect of sex (equivalent formulation). The way of identifying effects is totally different from what we have proposed above, so we named them differently (Table 2).

Table 2 Typology of sex S, social environment E and gender G effects on a health outcome Y

As with the previous strategy, some limitations are found. Firstly, we simply shift the problem of measuring a complex concept with one or several variables from the realm of gender to that of the social environment. Secondly, even if the WOGTES was large and the TEG null, cannot to conclude that Y are not at all influence by gender, because the measured effect would depend on the social environment variable chosen, and because a perfect non-gendered group of reference is not realistic (except some special cases like sex-blind in utero environment). Thirdly, an important condition for the successful use of this strategy would be to have a very socially heterogeneous population in order to estimate the variability of the gender effect across social groups by this TEG. On the other hand, the interest of this strategy is to be more compatible with the population-level nature of gender, considered as a sex–environment interaction or taking into account sex as a modifier of the effect of the environment. This approach makes it possible not to define a measure of gender which, even if defined in a study in a contextual way and with all precautions, always runs the risk of being generalized and essentialized afterwards.

Discussion

Choosing and conducting the strategy

We identified two methodological strategies to explore the mechanisms of health differences between men and women: (a) defining and measuring gender as a social mediator of sex or (b) defining and measuring gender as a sex–environment interaction. Here, we provide different arguments to help decide between them and recommendations to conduct them.

  1. (1)

    A fundamental criterion to select is the type of study population: strategy (a) is more appropriate if socio-cultural characteristics of the population are rather homogeneous; and strategy (b) is more appropriate if socio-cultural characteristics of the population are heterogeneous.

  2. (2)

    According to the research question, specify the chosen strategy and, with the help of tools such as directed acyclic graphs, define the specific effect to be estimated and the necessary methodological precautions [27].

  3. (3)

    Define required variables:

  • If strategy (a) is chosen, use available data and mechanistic assumptions to define variables for defining a gender score, based on the distribution of these variables according to sex in the study population (gender diagnosis), or keep the social characteristics of interest separated if methodologically possible.

  • If strategy (b) is chosen, define a summary variable of the social environment in the sample and globally assess the variability of differences between men and women according to this variable in the study population (e.g., income gap, employment access gap, age of first child, etc.). Define a reference group.

  1. (4)

    Discuss the limitations of the chosen method and interpret the results with these precautions.

  • If strategy (a) is chosen, evaluate and discuss the share of the gender phenomenon captured by the individual gender variable(s) and the share of non-gender-related phenomena also possibly captured (variability of the measurement depending on other social characteristics for example).

  • If strategy (b) is chosen, evaluate and discuss the share of the gender phenomenon captured: is the population sufficiently heterogeneous? Has it been possible to characterize the different socio-cultural groups with the "social environment" variable? How and how much are the various socio-cultural groups differently gendered?

  1. (5)

    Gender effects could then be qualitatively explored through: (i) the outcomes: for what health outcome do we observe a social mediation or a sex–environment interaction? (ii) the groups linked to the social environment: between which social groups do we observe or do not observe a difference in the sex effect? How and how much are these groups visibly gendered? (iii) the mediators: through what mediators does the gender effect or the interaction effect pass?

Limitations

These strategies must be understood within the defined perspective of understanding the mechanisms of differences between men and women in health. Studies that focus specifically on intersex, transgender, transsexual populations would require other approaches that are not described here. These strategies are also suited to a comprehensive and exploratory approach to the issue only: it seeks to explore the nature (biological or social) of observed differences between men and women when it does not seem trivial. The construction of a gender score seeks to capture a latent phenomenon, but does not necessarily imply that the variables used for this score are the risk factors for the health outcome. No a priori assumptions are therefore made on the type of specific exposures involved, which could be modified from a public health perspective. This objective could come in a second step, once the involvement of social mechanisms would have been identified.

Since the gender phenomenon is, as we have reiterated, contextual, the score constructed in a study on a specific population cannot be directly transposed to another population. The use of this kind of score can lead to the conclusion that a difference in health between men and women is, at least partly, explained by social mechanisms. However, the estimated size of the effect of these mechanisms could not be generalized to other populations, since the ways in which gender pressure performed in these other populations are likely to differ and therefore to influence health outcomes differently. The alternative approach, based on the study of sex–environment interaction, might more easily avoid the risk of essentializing differences, since it is precisely based on the variability of the involved processes. It may also make it possible to capture population phenomena by characterizing groups from the level of gender inequalities observed within them. These phenomena are otherwise difficult to capture with epidemiological methods based on individual-level variables, while it is a central aspect of understanding the gender process, based not only on the sex-differentiated norms prescription, but also on their interrelated hierarchical relations.

We argue that a causal analysis framework can guide us to refine our objectives, assumptions and conduct more rigorous analyses. It is from these methods that our approach has been built. However, this method has some drawbacks, including the need to define, for each factor, its "counterfactual". Firstly, this can give the impression that a binary categorization is unavoidable or that we are reinforcing it: if I was not born a female, it is because I was born a male; if I am not socialized as a woman, it is because I am socialized as a man. In practice, this may correspond to the way in which, in a gendered society based on a bipolarization of classifications, people are actually exposed or not to a kind of socialization. But this binarity must not be essentialized. A continuous masculinity (or femininity) score could also be constructed, its counterfactual formulation would be: "being socialized in a very masculine way" versus "being socialized in a less masculine way", without considering feminine socialization as the exact symmetry of this measure. Secondly and most importantly, definition of counterfactuals is constrained by reality since models are estimated from what is observed. So, it will usually not be possible to compare sex health differences observed in a given population with sex health differences “that would be observed in a population that would not know the gender phenomenon”, when only this comparison would really meet our objective. Similarly, the effect of a sex-independent gendered socialization will not be optimally measured, because, in our societies, moving from a female-socialization to a male-socialization for a female individual will never be equivalent to moving from a female-socialization to a male-socialization for a male individual. Perfect counterfactuals do not exist where gender is concerned.

Conclusion

Both analytical strategies have limitations relative to the impossibility of reducing a complex concept to a single or a few measures, and of capturing the entire effect of the phenomenon. However, these approaches, supported by causal framework, could lead to more accurate analyses of the mechanisms underlying health differences between men and women, and may ultimately limit the gender bias encountered in epidemiological and clinical research studies.

Perspectives and significance

In this article, we clarified concepts and measures of sex and gender. We argue that directed acyclic graph, used in causal framework to clearly formulate a priori assumption about links and direction of links, clarifies what is measured and reduces methodological bias. We proposed two analytical strategies depending on how we measure gender: as a social mediator of sex effect or as a sex–environment interaction. We provide different arguments to help decide between the first or the second strategy. Both analytical strategies have limitations relative to the impossibility of reducing a complex concept to a single or a few measures, and of capturing the entire effect of the phenomenon. Despite these limitations, these approaches, supported by causal framework, could lead to improve the coherence between complex SHS concepts and the analytical approach in epidemiology in the exploration of mechanisms underlying health differences between men and women.

Availability of data and materials

Not applicable, no data used.

Notes

  1. Defined as a discipline which manipulates quantitative or categorical variables defined at individual level to measure statistical associations, causal relations and mediation paths at populational level, with the objective of understanding etiological mechanisms behind health or disease.

References

  1. Hammarström A, Johansson K, Annandale E, Ahlgren C, Aléx L, Christianson M, Elwér S, et al. Central gender theoretical concepts in health research: the state of the art. J Epidemiol Commun Health. 2014;68(2):185–90. https://doi.org/10.1136/jech-2013-202572.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  2. Johnson JL, Greaves L, Repta R. Better science with sex and gender: facilitating the use of a sex and gender-based analysis in health research. Int J Equity Health. 2009;8:14. https://doi.org/10.1186/1475-9276-8-14.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  3. King BM. Point: a call for proper usage of “gender” and “sex” in biomedical publications. Am J Physiol Regulat Integr Compar Physiol. 2010;298(6):R1700-1701. https://doi.org/10.1152/ajpregu.00694.2009.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Nowatzki N, Grant KR. Sex is not enough: the need for gender-based analysis in health research. Health Care Women Int. 2011;32(4):263–77. https://doi.org/10.1080/07399332.2010.519838.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Tadiri CP, Raparelli V, Abrahamowicz M, Kautzy-Willer A, Kublickiene K, Herrero MT, Norris CM, Pilote L. Methods for prospectively incorporating gender into health sciences research. J Clin Epidemiol. 2021;129:191–7. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jclinepi.2020.08.018.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Doyal L. Sex, Gender, and Health: The Need for a New Approach. BMJ. 2001;323(7320):1061–3. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.323.7320.1061.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  7. Greaves L. Why put gender and sex into health research? In: Designing and conducting gender, sex, & health research. SAGE Publications, Inc: Thousand Oaks; 2012, 3‑14. https://doi.org/10.4135/9781452230610.

  8. Oertelt-Prigione S, Parol R, Krohn S, Preißner R, Regitz-Zagrosek V. Analysis of sex and gender-specific research reveals a common increase in publications and marked differences between disciplines. BMC Med. 2010;8(1):70. https://doi.org/10.1186/1741-7015-8-70.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  9. Hammarström A, Annandale E. A conceptual muddle: an empirical analysis of the use of ‘sex’ and ‘gender’ in ‘gender-specific medicine’ journals. PLoS ONE. 2012;7(4): e34193. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0034193.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  10. Krieger N. Genders, sexes, and health: what are the connections–and why does it matter? Int J Epidemiol. 2003;32(4):652–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Madsen TE, Bourjeily G, Hasnain M, Jenkins M, Morrison MF, Sandberg K, Tong IL, Trott J, Werbinski JL, McGregor AJ. Sex- and gender-based medicine: the need for precise terminology. Gender Genome. 2017;1(3):122–8. https://doi.org/10.1089/gg.2017.0005.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Ristvedt SL. The evolution of gender. JAMA Psychiat. 2014;71(1):13–4. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2013.3199.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Oakley A. Sex, gender and society. London: Temple Smith; 1972.

    Google Scholar 

  14. Fausto-Sterling A. The five sexes. Sciences. 1993;33(2):20–4. https://doi.org/10.1002/j.2326-1951.1993.tb03081.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Laqueur T. La Fabrique du sexe: Essai sur le corps et le genre en Occident. Paris: Gallimard; 1992.

    Google Scholar 

  16. Canadian Institutes of Health Research. Consulté le. https://www.cihr-irsc-igh-isfh.ca/. Accessed 1 mars 2022.

  17. Kelly-Irving M, et Cyrille Delpierre. The embodiment dynamic over the life course: a case for examining cancer aetiology. In: The Palgrave Handbook of Biology and Society, édité par Maurizio Meloni, John Cromby, Des Fitzgerald, et Stephanie Lloyd. Palgrave Macmillan: UK, 2018; 519‑40. https://doi.org/10.1057/978-1-137-52879-7_22.

  18. Kelly-Irving M, Delpierre C. Framework for understanding health inequalities over the life course: the embodiment dynamic and biological mechanisms of exogenous and endogenous origin. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2021;75(12):1181–6. https://doi.org/10.1136/jech-2021-216430.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Delphy C. L’ennemi principal. Tome 1: Économie politique du patriarcat. Paris, Syllepse, 1998.

  20. Durkheim É. Les règles de la méthode sociologique. Bibliothèque de philosophie contemporaine. Germer Baillière; 1895

  21. Bem SL. The measurement of psychological androgyny. J Consult Clin Psychol. 1974;42:155–62.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  22. Mahalik JR, Locke BD, Ludlow LH, Diemer MA, Scott RPJ, Gottfried M, Freitas G. Development of the conformity to masculine norms inventory. Psychol Men Masculinity. 2003;4:3–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Lippa RA, Martin LR, Friedman HS. Gender-related individual differences and mortality in the terman longitudinal study: is masculinity hazardous to your health? Pers Soc Psychol Bull. 2000;26(12):1560–70. https://doi.org/10.1177/01461672002612010.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Pelletier R, Ditto B, Pilote L. A composite measure of gender and its association with risk factors in patients with premature acute coronary syndrome. Psychosom Med. 2015;77(5):517–26. https://doi.org/10.1097/PSY.0000000000000186.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Smith PM, Koehoorn M. Measuring gender when you don’t have a gender measure: constructing a gender index using survey data. Int J Equity Health. 2016;15(1):82. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12939-016-0370-4.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  26. Pearl J. Causality. Models, reasoning, and inference. 2nd ed. New York: Cambridge University Press; 2009.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  27. Tennant PW, Murray EJ, Arnold KF, Berrie L, Fox MP, Gadd SC, Harrison WJ, et al. Use of directed acyclic graphs (DAGs) to identify confounders in applied health research: review and recommendations. Int J Epidemiol. 2021;50:620–32. https://doi.org/10.1093/ije/dyaa213.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. VanderWeele TJ. Explanation in causal inference. Methods for mediation and interaction. New York: Oxford University Press; 2015.

    Google Scholar 

  29. VanderWeele TJ. Mediation analysis: a practitioner’s guide. Annu Rev Public Health. 2016;37(17):32.

    Google Scholar 

  30. Mead, Margaret. 1963. Moeurs et sexualité en Océanie. Plon.

  31. VanderWeele TJ, Knol MJ. A tutorial on interaction. Epidemiol Methods. 2014;3:33–72. https://doi.org/10.1515/em-2013-0005.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

HC receives funding from the GINCO project ANR-19-CE41-0014. MKI receives funding from Horizon 2020 European research Council (Gendhi-Synergy grant agreement SGY2019-856478).

Funding

This research received no specific grant from any funding agency in the public, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors. HC receives funding from the GINCO project ANR-19-CE41-0014. MKI receives funding from Horizon 2020 European research Council (Gendhi-Synergy grant agreement SGY2019-856478).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

HC: conceptualization, methodology, writing (original draft preparation, review, and editing). AS: validation, review. BL: methodology, supervision, review. MKI: conceptualization, supervision, review. All authors have read the paper and they have agreed with the manuscript submission. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Hélène Colineaux.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Not applicable.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

No financial relationships with any organization that might have an interest in the submitted work; no other relationships or activities that could appear to have influenced the submitted work.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Colineaux, H., Soulier, A., Lepage, B. et al. Considering sex and gender in Epidemiology: a challenge beyond terminology. From conceptual analysis to methodological strategies. Biol Sex Differ 13, 23 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1186/s13293-022-00430-6

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s13293-022-00430-6

Keywords