Skip to main content

Perspective and strategy interactively modulate sex differences in a 3D navigation task

Abstract

Sex differences in navigation performance have been attributed to sex differences in information processing during navigation. Perspective refers to the viewpoint of the navigator, with previous work suggesting that men tend to use an allocentric perspective, while women tend to use an egocentric perspective during navigation. Furthermore, different navigation strategies may be used when moving from point A to B, with previous work suggesting that men tend to use a Euclidian strategy, while women tend to use a landmark-based strategy. However, it has not been studied whether perspective and strategy affect sex differences in navigation interactively or independently of each other. The present study aimed to investigate the interactive effects of perspective and strategy on sex differences in a 3D navigation task. In different levels of the task, perspective and strategy were modulated in a 2 × 2 design via different instructions. Potential mediating effects of video gaming experience and sex hormone levels were addressed. We found that men outperformed women in all levels of the navigation task. However, the male advantage was more pronounced using the allocentric perspective compared to the egocentric perspective. When using the allocentric perspective, men showed better performance using a Euclidian strategy while women showed better performance using a landmark-based strategy. The strategy did not modulate performance under the egocentric perspective. Accordingly, sex differences in navigation were interactively modulated by perspective and strategy. These effects were not explained by sex differences in video gaming experience or sex hormone levels.

Introduction

Virtual navigation tasks have been used to study different forms of navigation [1,2,3]. Goal-directed navigation, i.e., traveling to a destination, is the most intensely studied form of navigation in humans [4]. According to Spiers and Barry [5], goal-directed navigation depends on three aspects: (i) the properties of the environment itself, (ii) our knowledge about the environment, and (iii) the navigation strategies we employ. Virtual navigation tasks have the advantage that they allow us to control the first two aspects, i.e., the properties of the environment and participants’ knowledge of the environment in order to study more explicitly the navigation strategies subjects employ. The term strategy has been used somewhat inconsistently across the navigation literature, and various ways to distinguish different strategies from one another have been utilized [6,7,8]. The most prominent distinction is probably the distinction between hippocampus-dependent place strategies and caudate-dependent response strategies [8,9,10]. Note, however, that this distinction has been used in studies on spatial learning, i.e., the strategies are a means to acquire and retrieve information about an environment. Afterward, participants are required to navigate the environment which they got acquainted with. Different terminologies have been used to describe strategies for navigating new environments. These differences vary in linguistic or cartographic information [6, 7]. Nevertheless, there is a consensus on the existence of varying strategies. These strategies differ in how information about the environment is utilized to reach a destination.

Navigating an environment requires two types of spatial information: (i) a sense of direction traveled and (ii) a sense of the distances traveled. Directions can be judged by either an allocentric or an egocentric reference frame [4, 11]. Egocentric navigation judges direction (but also distances) relative to the navigators own position (“left,” “right”), i.e., from the navigators first-person perspective. Allocentric navigation judges direction independent of the navigators’ position, e.g., along cardinal directions (“north,” “south,” etc.). During egocentric navigation, the coordinates of the navigator remain stable, while the coordinates of the target change. During allocentric navigation, on the other hand, the coordinates of the target remain stable, while the coordinates of the navigator change. Distances can be judged metrically or topologically [4]. Metric knowledge involves precise information about the absolute distances (e.g., 35 m) between landmarks in the environment or between the navigator and the target, and is thus often described in Euclidian terms (not to be confused with the Euclidian distance to a target!). Topological knowledge involves less precise information about the relative position between landmarks in the environment or between the navigator and the target. Thus, it is often described in landmark-based terms (e.g., “next to the church”).

These different types of direction (allocentric vs. egocentric) and distance (metric vs. topological/Euclidian vs. landmark-based) information play an important role in the interpretation of sex differences in navigation. While women on average outperform men in verbal and episodic memory tasks, men on average outperform women in visuospatial tasks, like mental rotation, and navigation [12,13,14,15,16]. In fact, navigation is one of the cognitive domains that elicits the most robust sex differences across different cultures [7, 12, 17, 18]. Note that most studies of sex differences in navigation focus on goal-directed navigation in new environments. Men’s precedence in navigation tasks is reflected in fewer navigational errors and faster response times when traveling to a destination. The most robust and consistent sex differences have been found in 3D environments [19,20,21,22], while women made fewer errors in 2D navigation tasks [7, 12, 23]. It has thus been claimed that sex differences emerge more consistently in navigation tasks higher in cognitive demands [24, 25]. Furthermore, sex differences in navigation (and other spatial tasks) have been related to training, e.g., higher experience with virtual environments due to video gaming in men [26,27,28], or hormonal factors, e.g., higher testosterone levels in men ([29,30,31,32,33,34], but see [21, 35,36,37]).

Most importantly, sex differences in navigation have been attributed to the use of different navigation strategies by men and women [6, 17, 23, 38, 39]. Various terms have been used to describe these different navigation strategies in men and women (e.g., survey vs. route-based [6], allocentric vs. egocentric [7], Euclidian vs. landmark-based [40]), none corresponding to the terms used for spatial learning (i.e., place vs. response strategies). We here reserve the term “strategy” to distinguish between navigation based on metric (Euclidian) information and landmark-based topological navigation. There is, however, some consistency in the types of spatial information these strategies refer to. During goal-directed navigation, men appear to utilize allocentric and metric information about the environment at a higher rate than women, while women appear to utilize egocentric and topological information at a higher rate than men [6, 7, 41]. This is, for instance, reflected in the way participants remember a map or environment, or the linguistic terms used by participants when describing a path through an environment [17, 23, 38, 42,43,44,45,46]. When participants memorize a map, women recall significantly more landmarks off and on the route, whereas men recall more Euclidian parameters [23, 38, 42, 43]. When participants are asked to describe the path they take, men use more allocentric and Euclidian terms, while women use more egocentric and landmark-based terms [17, 38, 44,45,46]. Furthermore, men and women differ in their self-reports of navigation strategies. While men report taking shortcuts and focusing on distal landmarks to navigate, women report following well-learned routes and depending on local landmarks [6, 24, 38, 47]. Several studies indicate that women perform better when following egocentric directions, while men perform better when following allocentric directions [6, 7]. Furthermore, multiple studies indicate that women make fewer errors during navigation and show comparable performance to men when more landmark information is available, i.e., in landmark-rich environments [19, 20, 48]. In a 2D matrix navigation task, Saucier et al. [7] phrased directions to a target on the matrix in either allocentric and Euclidian terms or egocentric and landmark-based terms. They observed a male superiority in performance when allocentric and Euclidian terms were used, but a female superiority when egocentric and landmark-based terms were used.

However, so far, no study on sex differences in navigation has studied the two types of information (direction and distance) independently. Rather allocentric directions have been confounded with Euclidian terms to describe distances while egocentric directions have been confounded with landmark-based terms to describe distances [24]. Thus, it remains unclear, whether sex differences in navigation are attributable to the use of different reference frames/perspectives or different strategies to judge distances in the environment. In order to better understand sex differences in navigation strategies, it is thus necessary to dissociate these two types of information. The matrix navigation task developed by Saucier et al. [7] is ideal for that goal.

In a previous study, Scheuringer and Pletzer [40] implemented an adapted version of Saucier’s 2D-version navigation task [21] varying the terms used to describe direction and distance in a 2 × 2 design. However, due to the 2D implementation of the task with north always facing upwards, navigation performance was substantially confounded with mental rotation. Most problematically, participants had to mentally rotate the environment, if egocentric directions were given. Accordingly, Scheuringer and Pletzer [40] observed no female advantage with egocentric directions. In the present study, we employ a 3D adaptation of the Saucier matrix navigation task, but like Scheuringer and Pletzer [40], we use instructions that vary the terms used to describe direction and distance in a 2 × 2 design.

We hypothesize that sex differences in navigation performance depend on the phrasing of directions, in such a way that:

  1. 1.

    Sex differences favoring males will be larger with allocentric directions compared to egocentric directions since allocentric directions force participants to obtain an allocentric perspective, while egocentric directions force participants to obtain an egocentric perspective.

  2. 2.

    Sex differences favoring males will be larger when distances are described in Euclidian terms compared to landmark-based terms since Euclidian terms force participants to use a more metric representation of the environment (Euclidian strategy), while landmark-based terms allow participants to use a topological representation (landmark-based strategy).

Accordingly, we expect the largest sex differences in the condition combining allocentric directions with Euclidian terms (interactive effect). Since previous studies have attributed sex differences in navigation to training or hormonal factors [35,36,37,38], we additionally address potential mediating effects of video gaming experience and sex hormone levels.

Method

Participants

Eighty-four healthy participants (42 men; 42 women), aged 18–36 years, were recruited for this study. Exclusion criteria were physical, endocrine, and mental illness; hormonal contraception or medication; and left-handedness. A regular menstrual cycle [49] was required for female participants, ranging from 21 to 35 days. Previous studies demonstrate that navigation strategies also vary along the menstrual cycle with increased use of landmark information during the high-hormone luteal cycle phase [40, 50]. Accordingly, in the present study, all women were tested during their mid-luteal cycle phase, which was determined as follows: Based on participants’ self-reports about the onset of last period and cycle duration, ovulation was calculated 14 days before the assumed onset of their next period. The testing session was scheduled 3–10 days after ovulation, i.e., 10–3 days before the expected onset of the next menses. Three women were excluded, because of extremely low progesterone values (< 48 pg/ml, see below), suggesting a mismatch between the calculated cycle phase and hormone values. Furthermore, one man was excluded due to estradiol values exceeding the group mean by more than three standard deviations (SD). This resulted in a final sample of 80 participants, 41 men (mean age 24.29, SD = 2.40, range 20–29 years) and 39 women (mean age 23.17, SD = 4.97, range 18–36 years) with an average cycle duration of 29.19 days (SD = 2.79 days), who were on average tested on day 22.23 (SD = 3.53) of their menstrual cycle. Age did not differ significantly between men and women (t = 1.30, p = .197), neither did education (t = − 1.22 p = .226) nor IQ (t = .890, p = .376; men: mean IQ 108.36, SD = 9.18; women: mean IQ 105.10, SD = 21.70). All participants had a minimum of 8 years of secondary education and had passed the general qualification for university entrance.

Ethics statement

All participants gave their informed written consent to participate in the study. The methods used conformed to the Code of Ethics of the World Medical Association (Declaration of Helsinki).

The institutional guidelines of the University of Salzburg (Statutes of the University of SalzburgFootnote 1) state in §163 (1) that it is necessary to seek ethical approval for research on human subjects if the physical or psychological integrity is affected, the right for privacy or other important rights or interests of the subjects or their dependents are confounded. Paragraph §163 (2) states that it is the decision and responsibility of the PI to decide whether (1) applies to a study or not. Therefore, no ethical approval for this study was sought out. Non-invasive methods were used on healthy adult volunteers, who willingly gave their informed consent to participate. Accordingly, (1) did not apply. Data were processed in anonymized/de-identified form. Participants were assigned a subject ID (v001, v002, etc.), when physically present at the lab, which was used throughout the study.

Navigation task

For this study, 20 task environments of a 3D navigation task were created using Unreal Engine 4 Version 8.1, building on a 2D version employed by Scheuringer and Pletzer [40], which was adapted from Saucier et al. [7]. Each task environment was a checkerboard consisting of 100 squares (10 × 10) plus 1 starting square positioned on the outside (Fig. 1). One of the ten real-life landmarks (tree, bridge, stairs, house, church, bench, boulder, street light, fence, flowers) was placed in each square. The order of landmarks was different for each task environment. However, in each task environment, each item only appeared once in each row and column, meaning each item could be found ten times in each task environment. That way, rows and columns share similarities with streets which participants may encounter in real life with the exception that each landmark occurs only once in each row/column. This restriction is necessary to not confound Euclidian and landmark-based distance information (see below). Accordingly, each task environment was new to the participants, similar to a new city or a new part of town. In each level of the navigation task, participants had to navigate one of these new environments. Each level started with a countdown (“three, two, one, go”), while participants were positioned on the starting square facing the new environment. After the countdown, participants used the arrow keys to navigate the environment. Their task was to reach a target location following a path, which was indicated by three lines of instructions (Fig. 2). Four conditions were characterized by different phrasings of the instructions, which modulated the terms to describe direction (here: perspective) and the terms to describe distances (here: strategy) in a 2 × 2 design: allocentric Euclidian (“go east for 4 blocks”), allocentric landmark-based (“go east until you reach the tree”), egocentric Euclidian (“turn right and go for 4 blocks”), and egocentric landmark-based (“turn right and go until you reach the tree”). There were four training levels, i.e., one for each condition. Table 1 lists example directions for each condition. After the training, participants completed 16 task levels, i.e., 4 task levels per condition. The order of conditions was pseudo-randomized throughout the task. At the beginning of each level, i.e., after the countdown, participants were informed which cardinal directions they were currently facing. Starting directions were counterbalanced across conditions, i.e., among the four items per condition, one started facing north, one facing south, one facing east, and one facing west. Accordingly, participants started four times in each cardinal direction. Each path encompassed 15 squares and 2 turns. Without moving backward, there were four possible patterns of movement in the environment: two left turns or two right turns resulted in a U-shaped path, and a left turn followed by a right turn or a right turn followed by a left turn resulted in a Z-shaped path. The pattern of movement, as well as the number of squares before the first and second turn, was counterbalanced across conditions to ensure that participants could not predict the path based on experience from previous levels. Participants only advanced to the next level when the target location was reached and the correct location was confirmed by pressing the space bar. When the final destination of each level was successfully reached, and the space bar was pressed, participants had to identify north. A screen with four arrows pointing toward each edge of the screen appeared. By pressing the arrow keys on their keyboard, participants could indicate where they assumed north was. There were no maximum time restrictions for the navigation nor the north question.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Layout of an exemplary level of the navigation task. Start field with an arrow indicating the cardinal direction (bottom left corner). 10 × 10 matrix with each item appearing once in each row and column

Fig. 2
figure 2

Example of directions for one level of the navigation task. The instructions were presented in the bottom left corner; a participant is navigating through the virtual world. Examples of items placed in the world are visible such as fence, tree, house, church, boulders, and flowers

Table 1 Training level directions

The measures of performance navigation time in seconds (navigation time—NT) was recorded by timing the participant from the moment the “go” appeared up to the moment the space bar was activated at the correct location. Accuracy (was the correct arrow pressed which represents north? Yes or no was measured) and reaction time in seconds, from the moment the identifying north screen appeared until an arrow key was pressed, were also recorded.

In order to correctly identify north after reaching the target location, it was required to consider all turns made since starting. It was assumed that navigation time reflected navigation abilities, while performance in identifying north reflected mental rotation abilities when using different perspectives and strategies.

Procedure

Participants completed the 3D navigation task as part of a larger study. Participants were asked not to eat, drink, or smoke for 30 min before arriving at the laboratory. When participants arrived at the computer lab of the University of Salzburg, they were asked to rinse out their mouths and fill out the informed consent form and a screening questionnaire pertaining to the exclusion criteria and habits (sleeping, eating, smoking, etc.). Afterward, i.e., approximately 15 min after arriving at the lab, they gave the first of three saliva samples and completed two computerized tasks addressing attention and numerical processing, respectively. These two tasks, which took approximately 30 min, were always completed in the same order and will be described elsewhere. Following this, the second saliva sample was taken for sex hormone analysis. Then, the 3D navigation task was completed by each participant, followed by the last saliva sample. The duration of the navigation task varied across participants depending on their performance but took approximately 30 min. In the end, participants indicated their computer gaming experience on a scale of 1–9. They were specifically instructed to indicate how much experience they had with ego-shooter video games, 1 meaning no experience at all and 9 being professional gamers. Furthermore, participants completed the ten-item APM screening (advanced progressive matrices [51]) as implemented in the Vienna Test System [52] as a coarse measure of general intelligence to ensure that there were no substantial differences in IQ between men and women and that all participants had sufficient cognitive abilities to recognize, process, and handle new patterns as presented during the navigation task. Participants were debriefed and received either course credit or 10 € for participating.

Hormone analyses

Each participant gave three saliva samples (compare the “Procedure” section). Saliva samples were stored at − 20 °C and centrifuged twice at 3000 rpm for 15 and 10 min, respectively, prior to hormone assessment. The three samples of each participant were pooled before hormone analysis in order to assess average hormone concentrations over the whole session, thereby controlling for fluctuations related to salivary production and ensuring the reliability of hormone assessment. Testosterone, 17β-estradiol, and progesterone were assessed from the pooled sample of each participant using salivary ELISA kits by DeMediTec.

Participants with hormone values deviating by more than three standard deviations from the group mean were excluded. In addition, we expected progesterone values for women to fall within a normal range for the luteal cycle phase. As recommended by the DeMediTec kit instructions, we established this range for our laboratory based on an unrelated sample of 60 women tested with the DeMediTec salivary progesterone ELISA kit in three menstrual cycle phases (compare Pletzer et al., accepted). In all women, progesterone levels were higher during the luteal phase compared to the other cycle phases. While there was substantial variation in the luteal phase values, none of the women displayed a luteal progesterone value below 48 pg/ml. Taking into account the assay sensitivity of 5 pg/ml, a progesterone cutoff of 43 pg/ml was established for inclusion in a luteal phase sample.

Statistical analyses

Navigation time (NT) and accuracy, as well as reaction time when identifying north, were analyzed in the context of linear mixed effects models (lmes) using the lmer function of lme4 package (Version 1.1-18-1) [53] of statistics software RStudio 1.1.456 In all models, the participant number was modeled as a random factor. Furthermore, backward elimination of non-significant interactions was applied to all models using the step function of the lmerTest package (Version 3.0-1). The following models were evaluated:

In a first step, we addressed sex differences in the dependent variables and their modulation by perspective and strategy by introducing the interactive effect of sex*perspective*strategy as a fixed effect in the model. To control for training effects, a level was additionally entered as a covariate in the model (e.g., NT ~ 1|PNr + level + sex*perspective*strategy). If significant sex differences were found, potential mediating effects of ego-shooter gaming experience were addressed in a second step using three models. First, it was established whether sex affected ego-shooter experience (ego-shooter ~ sex). Second, it was established whether ego-shooter experience affected navigation time (NT ~ 1|PNr + ego-shooter*perspective*strategy). Third, both sex and ego-shooter experience were entered in the model (NT ~ 1|PNr + sex*ego-shooter*perspective*strategy). In the third step, the same procedure was used to assess potential mediating effects of sex hormone levels.

In all models, both, the dependent and continuous independent variables were z-standardized using the scale function. Therefore, the coefficients b of fixed effects in the models represent a standardized effect size based on standard deviations, similar to Cohen’s d.

Data availability

The datasets generated and/or analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.

Results

Sex differences in navigation time and their modulation by perspective and strategy

To evaluate sex differences in navigation performance and whether they were modulated by perspective and strategy, we applied a lme on the dependent variable navigation time modulating participant number as a random factor and level, as well as the interactive effects of sex*perspective*strategy as fixed effects (NT ~ 1|PNr + level + sex*perspective*strategy).

Level had a significant negative effect on navigation time (b = − 0.10, SEb = 0.03, t(1216) = − 3.88, p < .001). Participants completed the levels faster the more levels they had completed.

The main effects of sex were significant (b = 0.60, SEb = 0.12, t(80) = 4.86, p < .001). Men showed a faster navigation time (40.46 s) compared to women (52.82 s). The main effects of perspective (b = − 0.13, SEb = 0.10, t(1216) = − 1.29, p = .196) and strategy (b = 0.13, SEb = 0.10, t(1216) = 1.26, p = .205) as well as the perspective*strategy interaction (b = − 0.21, SEb = 0.14, t(1216) = − 1.49, p = .136) did not reach significance. Sex showed significant interactions with both perspective (b = − 0.37, SEb = 0.14, t(1216) = − 2.56, p = .011) and strategy (b = − 0.38, SEb = 0.14, t(1216) = − 2.69, p = .007). Furthermore, there was a significant three-way interaction of sex*perspective*strategy (b = 0.43, SEb = 0.20, t(1216) = 2.14, p = .033) (Fig. 3).

Fig. 3
figure 3

Navigation time for all groups and conditions. A threefold interaction between sex, perspective, and strategy was observed. Men reach the target location faster than women in all conditions. Both men and women reach the target location faster with egocentric directions compared to allocentric directions. The effect of perspective is larger in women compared to men. No strategy differences in performance were observed for the egocentric perspective. For the allocentric perspective, men reached the target location faster with the Euclidian strategy, while women reached the target location faster with the landmark-based strategy

In order to resolve these interactions, separate models were run for men and women (NT ~ 1| PNr + perspective*strategy), for the allocentric and egocentric perspective (NT ~ 1| PNr + sex*strategy), and for the Euclidian compared to the landmark-based strategy (NT ~ 1|PNr + sex*perspective). For both men and women, a significant main effect of perspective was observed (men: b = − 0.25, SEb = 0.10, t(627) = − 2.47, p = 0.014; women: b = − 0.48, SEb = 0.10, t(590) = − 4.64, p < 0.001), indicating faster navigation with egocentric compared to allocentric directions. The effect size was much larger in women. The main effect of strategy was only significant in women (b = − 0.22, SEb = 0.10, t(590) = − 2.08, p = .038), indicating faster navigation with a landmark-based strategy compared to the Euclidian strategy. In men, the main effect of strategy did not reach significance, but the effect size indicates faster navigation time with the Euclidian compared to the landmark-based strategy (b = 0.17, SEb = 0.10, t(627) = 1.65, p = 0.100). The perspective*strategy interaction did not reach significance in either men or women (all |b| < 0.25, all SEb < 0.14, all t(627) > 1.74, all p < 0.082).

For both perspectives, the main effect of sex was significant (allo: b = 0.60, SEb = 0.16, t(80) = 3.71, p < 0.001; ego: b = 0.23, SEb = 0.08, t(80) = 2.94, p = 0.004), indicating faster navigation time for men compared to women. The effect size of sex differences was larger for allocentric compared to the egocentric directions. The main effect of strategy was non-significant (both |b| < 0.10, both SEb > 0.09, both |t| < 1.01, both p > 0.31). However, we observed a significant interaction of sex*strategy with allocentric directions (b = − 0.38, SEb = 0.19, t(570) = − 2.06, p = .040), which was absent with the egocentric directions (b = 0.05, SEb = 0.07, t(567) = 0.72, p = .470). With allocentric directions, women showed significantly faster navigation times for the landmark compared to the Euclidian strategy, while men showed faster navigation times for the Euclidian compared to the landmark strategy.

For both strategies, navigation time was faster for men compared to women (eucl: b = 0.60, SEb = 0.13, t(80) = 4.55, p < 0.001; landm: b = 0.22, SEb = 0.11, t(80) = 1.96, p = 0.053). The effect size of sex differences was larger for the Euclidian compared to landmark strategy. Navigation time was faster for egocentric compared to allocentric perspective in both strategies (both |b| > 0.20, both SEb > 0.08, both |t| > 1.78, both p < 0.07). However, we observed a significant interaction of sex*perspective in the Euclidian strategy (b = − 0.36, SEb = 0.16, t(568) = − 2.29, p = .02), which was absent in the landmark strategy (b = 0.06, SEb = 0.13, t(569) = 0.49, p = .617). Sex differences were larger for allocentric compared to egocentric directions only with the Euclidian strategy.

The mediating role of ego-shooter experience on sex differences in navigation time

In order to evaluate whether the sex influence on navigation performance was mediated by ego-shooter experience, the following steps were performed.

In the first step, we evaluated sex differences in ego-shooter experience. Sex had a significant effect on ego-shooter experience (b = − 1.17, SEb = 0.18, t(79) = − 6.40, p < .001), indicating higher ego-shooter experience in men compared to women.

In the second step, we evaluated the impact of ego-shooter experience on navigation performance. To that end, we applied a lme on the dependent variable navigation time modulating participant number as a random factor and level, as well as the interactive effects of egoshooter*perspective*strategy as fixed effects (NT ~ 1|PNr + level + egoshooter*perspective*strategy).

There were significant main effects of ego-shooter experience (b = − 0.22, SEb = 0.05, t(79) = − 4.40, p < .001) and perspective (b = − 0.31, SEb = 0.05, t(1205) = − 6.04, p < .001). The main effect of strategy did not reach significance and showed no significant interaction with perspective or ego-shooter experience. It was thus removed from the model. There was, however, a significant interaction between ego-shooter experience and perspective (b = 0.11, SEb = 0.05, t(1205) = 2.07, p = .038), indicating a smaller impact of the perspective effect in participants with larger ego-shooter experience.

In the third step, we evaluated, whether ego-shooter experience could explain the impact of sex on navigation performance, by entering both variables as fixed effects into a linear mixed model (NT ~ 1|PNr + level + sex*egoshooter*perspective*strategy).

In this model, the main effect of ego-shooter experience did not reach significance and did not interact with any other factor. It was thus removed from the model, resulting in a final model as described in the “Sex differences in navigation time and their modulation by perspective and strategy” section. Thus, rather than the sex difference in navigation performance being mediated by ego-shooter experience, the effect of ego-shooter experience on navigation performance is mediated by sex.

The mediating role of sex hormones on sex differences in navigation time

To evaluate whether sex influences on navigation performance were mediated via sex hormone levels, the following analyses were performed.

In the first step, we confirmed that men and women differed in their sex hormone levels. Men had significantly higher testosterone levels compared to women (b = − 1.19, SEb = 0.18, t(76) = − 6.42, p < .001), whereas women had significantly higher progesterone levels (b = 1.04, SEb = 0.20, t(75) = 5.26, p < .001) and, by trend, higher estradiol levels (b = 0.44, SEb = 0.23, t(75) = 1.95, p = .055) compared to men.

In the second step, we evaluated the impact of sex hormones on navigation performance. To that end, we applied lmes on the dependent variable navigation time modulating participant number as a random factor and level as well as the interactive effects of hormone*perspective*strategy as fixed effects (NT ~ 1|PNr + level + hormone*perspective*strategy). Testosterone and progesterone were significantly related to navigation time (testosterone: b = − 0.10, SEb = 0.05, t(76) = − 2.17, p = 0.034; progesterone: b = 0.12, SEb = 0.05, t(75) = 2.54, p = 0.013), but did not interact with perspective and strategy. Time to reach the target was lower in participants with higher testosterone and lower progesterone levels. Estradiol was not related to navigation time and did not interact with perspective and strategy; thus it was removed from the model.

In the third step, we evaluated whether sex hormone levels could explain the impact of sex on navigation performance by entering both variables as fixed effects into a linear mixed model (NT ~ 1|PNr + level + sex*hormone*perspective*strategy). In this model, none of the hormones showed a significant main effect and none of them interacted with any other factor. They were thus removed from the model. Thus, rather than the sex difference in navigation performance being mediated by sex hormone levels, the effects of sex hormone levels on navigation performance are mediated by sex.

Identifying north: accuracy and reaction time

In order to evaluate sex differences in the speed and accuracy of identifying north and whether it was modulated by perspective and strategy, we applied lmes on the dependent variables accuracy and reaction time modulating participant number as a random factor and the interactive effects of sex*perspective*strategy as fixed effects (north ~ 1|PNr + level + sex*perspective*strategy).

In both models, the main effect of sex was non-significant and did not interact with perspective or strategy. It was thus removed from the models. Level had a significant effect on both accuracy (b = 0.10, SEb = 0.03, t(1221) = 3.28, p = .001) and reaction time (b = − 0.27, SEb = 0.03, t(1220) = − 8.76, p < .001). Participants rated north more accurately and faster the more levels they had completed.

In the analysis of accuracy, the main effect of perspective (both b = − 0.32, SEb = 0.05, t(1221) = − 6.41, p < .001) was significant, indicating a higher accuracy with the allocentric (89%) compared to the egocentric perspective (76%). The main effects of sex and strategy, as well as all interactions, did not reach significance and were thus removed from the model (Fig. 4a).

Fig. 4
figure 4

Accuracy and reaction time in identifying north. a Accuracy: Accuracy in identifying north was significantly higher for the allocentric perspective compared to the egocentric perspective. No effect of strategy was observed. b Reaction time: The landmark strategy resulted in faster identifications for the allocentric perspective, whereas the Euclidian strategy resulted in faster identifications for the egocentrical perspective. Men and women did not differ in accuracy and reaction time when identifying north

In the analysis of reaction time, the main effect of perspective (b = 0.25, SEb = 0.05, t(1220) = 5.18, p < 0.001) and strategy were significant (b = − 0.12, SEb = 0.05, t(1220) = − 2.41, p = 0.016). The interaction between strategy and perspective was non-significant and thus removed from the model. These results indicate that north was identified faster with allocentric compared to egocentric directions and with the landmark compared to the Euclidian strategy (Fig. 4b).

Since no sex differences were observed in the ability to correctly identify north, modulating roles of ego-shooter experience and sex hormones were not addressed further.

Discussion

The present study aimed to investigate whether sex differences are modulated by strategy and perspective during a 3D navigation task. Since the allocentric perspective and Euclidian strategy both incorporate a more global view on navigation and egocentrical perspective and landmark-based strategy a more localized view on navigation, the following hypotheses were established:

  1. 1.

    Sex differences favoring males will be larger with allocentric directions compared to egocentric directions since allocentric directions force participants to obtain an allocentric perspective while egocentric directions force participants to obtain an egocentric perspective.

  2. 2.

    Sex differences favoring males will be larger when distances are described in Euclidian terms compared to landmark-based terms since Euclidian terms force participants to use a more metric representation of the environment (Euclidian strategy), while landmark-based terms allow participants to use a topological representation (landmark-based strategy).

Interactions between perspective and strategy were explored. Furthermore, we address the potential mediating effects of video gaming experience and sex hormone levels.

We observed that men find the target location significantly faster across all strategies and perspectives than women. This is in line with multiple previous findings, demonstrating a male superiority in spatial navigation [19,20,21,22]. It is, however, in contrast with previous findings, suggesting that a landmark-based strategy or egocentric perspective can reduce or reverse the sex difference in navigation performance [19, 23, 38, 48]. Specifically, Saucier et al. [7] demonstrated that women outperformed men in the matrix navigation task when directions were phrased in egocentric and landmark-based terms. Note, however, that they used a 2D version of the task, for which sex differences are generally smaller [7, 12, 23].

Our results, however, show that the modulation of sex differences in navigation by perspective and strategy was not as straightforward as hypothesized.

Regarding perspective, both male and female participants needed more time to locate the target when directions were phrased in allocentric rather than egocentric terms. The egocentric perspective allowed for the fastest navigation, irrespective of sex and strategy. This is in contrast to findings by Saucier et al. [7], who found that men performed better with allocentric compared to egocentric directions. Note, however, that in their study, egocentric directions were confounding with a landmark-based strategy and allocentric directions with a Euclidian strategy. It is also possible that the favorable use of perspectives is shaped by education and experience. Previously, navigating via maps required the use of allocentric perspectives; nowadays, most car navigation system or online mapping services use the egocentrical perspective and Euclidian strategy to navigate; therefore, people are exposed to this type of instruction more frequently, whereas the allocentric instruction only constantly occurs in a specialized field such as the military [54,55,56].

However, sex differences are stronger for the allocentric compared to the egocentric perspective, i.e., the allocentric perspective impaired performance in women more than in men. This is particularly apparent when focusing on the Euclidian strategy. Thus, while navigation performance in men is not facilitated by using the allocentric perspective as suggested by the literature, it is certainly the case that men are less impaired by the allocentric perspective than women.

Regarding strategy, sex differences were stronger for the Euclidian than for the landmark-based strategy. However, a sex*strategy interaction was only observed for the allocentric perspective, but not for the egocentric perspective. For allocentric directions, women showed an advantage with a landmark-based strategy, while men showed an advantage with a Euclidian strategy as hypothesized. It is possible that for the egocentric perspective, a ceiling effect was achieved, such that strategy differences were not picked up by performance measures. Egocentrical instructions are easier to understand and learned earlier in life, making navigation universally easier compared to the specific training needed to master the allocentric perspective [54,55,56,57,58,59]. This is in line with the claim by Coluccia and Louse that sex differences tend to appear only when the task is difficult [24, 25].

Notably, these findings were mediated neither by confounding variables like ego-shooter experience and training effects nor by sex hormone levels. Thus, sex differences in virtual navigation performance cannot solely be explained by men’s higher experience with virtual worlds. The fact that the results were also irrespective of sex hormone levels suggests that these differences in performance and cognitive strategies are stable and develop either under the influence of organizational effects of sex hormones, due to socialization, or other factors that are irrespective of activational effects of sex hormones. It is however also possible that methodological factors, like power considerations or the non-invasive assessment of hormones from saliva, prevented us from detecting potential relations to sex hormone levels. Notably, we were able to detect menstrual cycle-dependent changes in perspective and strategy in a previous version of the task [40].

No sex differences were observed when identifying north. For this question, the allocentric perspective was beneficial compared to the egocentric perspective, irrespective of sex. The instructions for the allocentric perspective are phrased using the cardinal directions (“north,” “south,” “east,” “west”), giving participants obvious pointers along the way indicating in which direction they are currently moving, which makes the decision easier. With the egocentric instruction, participants only register the current cardinal direction once when they leave the starting block and have to remember in which direction they started. For reaction times, a significant advantage in identifying north was also observed with landmark-based instructions. This suggests that both men and women anchor cardinal directions at landmarks in the environment (e.g., the church is north), such that landmark information facilitates a sense of one’s position with respect to the cardinal directions in the environment. While these results were observed for both men and women, they also suggest that spatial orientation in a virtual environment is modulated interactively by perspective and strategy and that these two factors should not be confounded with each other.

In summary, this is the first study to adequately disentangle previously confounded roles of perspective and strategy for sex differences in the navigation task. The results are in line with previous findings but suggest that perspective and strategy modulate sex differences in navigation performance interactively rather than independently. Thus, the 3D navigation task created for this study is particularly valuable for research on different navigation strategies used by men and women.

Notes

  1. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC5355435/ #fn01

References

  1. Warren WH, Rothman DB, Schnapp BH, Ericson JD. Wormholes in virtual space: from cognitive maps to cognitive graphs. Cognition. 2017;166:152–63.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  2. Gerber R, Kwan T. A phenomenographical approach to the study of pre adolescents’ use of maps in a wayfinding exercise in a suburban environment. J Environ Psychol. 1994;14(4):265–80.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Steck SD, Mallot HA. The role of global and local landmarks in virtual environment navigation. Presence. 2000;9(1):69–83.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Ekstrom AD, Spiers HJ, Bohbot VD, Rosenbaum RS. Human spatial navigation. New Jersey: Princeton University Press; 2018. p.24; 85.

  5. Spiers HJ, Barry C. Neural systems supporting navigation. Curr Opin Behav Sci. 2015;1:47–55.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Lawton CA. Gender differences in way-finding strategies: relationship to spatial ability and spatial anxiety. Sex Roles. 1994;30(11–12):765–79.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Saucier DM, Green SM, Leason J, MacFadden A, Bell S, Elias LJ. Are sex differences in navigation caused by sexually dimorphic strategies or by differences in the ability to use the strategies? Behav Neurosci. 2002;116(3):403.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Bohbot VD, McKenzie S, Konishi K, Fouquet C, Kurdi V, Schachar R, Boivin M, Robaey P. Virtual navigation strategies from childhood to senescence: evidence for changes across the life span. Front Aging Neurosci. 2012;4:28.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. McDonald RJ, White NM. Parallel information processing in the water maze: evidence for independent memory systems involving dorsal striatum and hippocampus. Behav Neural Biol. 1994;61(3):260–70.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  10. Packard MG, McGaugh JL. Double dissociation of fornix and caudate nucleus lesions on acquisition of two water maze tasks: further evidence for multiple memory systems. Behav Neurosci. 1992;106(3):439.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  11. Burgess N. Spatial memory: how egocentric and allocentric combine. Trends Cogn Sci. 2006;10(12):551–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Andreano JM, Cahill L. Sex influences on the neurobiology of learning and memory. Learn Mem. 2009;16(4):248–66.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Cochran KF, Wheatley GH. Ability and sex-related differences in cognitive strategies on spatial tasks. J Gen Psychol. 1989;116(1):43–55.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Förster J, Dannenberg L. GLOMOsys: a systems account of global versus local processing. Psychol Inq. 2010;21(3):175–97.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Kimchi R, Amishav R, Sulitzeanu-Kenan A. Gender differences in global–local perception? Evidence from orientation and shape judgments. Acta Psychol. 2009;130(1):64–71.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Razumnikova OM, Vol'f NV. Selection of visual hierarchical stimuli between global and local aspects in men and women. Fiziol Cheloveka. 2011;37(2):14–9.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Dabbs JM, Chang EL, Strong RA, Milun R. Spatial ability, navigation strategy, and geographic knowledge among men and women. Evol Huma Behav. 1998;19(2):89–98.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Silverman I, Choi J, Peters M. The hunter-gatherer theory of sex differences in spatial abilities: data from 40 countries. Arch Sex Behav. 2007;36(2):261–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Andersen NE, Dahmani L, Konishi K, Bohbot VD. Eye tracking, strategies, and sex differences in virtual navigation. Neurobiol Learn. 2012;97:81–9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2011.09.007 .

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Astur RS, Ortiz ML, Sutherland RJ. A characterization of performance by men and women in a virtual Morris water task: a large and reliable sex difference. Behav Brain Res. 1998;93(1–2):185–90.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  21. Moffat SD, Hampson E. A curvilinear relationship between testosterone and spatial cognition in humans: possible influence of hand preference. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 1996;21(3):323–37.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  22. Silverman I, Choi J, Mackewn A, Fisher M, Moro J, Olshansky E. Evolved mechanisms underlying wayfinding: further studies on the hunter-gatherer theory of spatial sex differences. Evol Hum Behav. 2000;21(3):201–13.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  23. Galea LA, Kimura D. Sex differences in route-learning. Personal Individ Differ. 1993;14(1):53–65.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Coluccia E, Louse G. Gender differences in spatial orientation: a review. J Environ Psychol. 2004;24:329–40.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Forcano L, Santamaría J, Mackintosh NJ, Chamizo VD. Single landmark learning: sex differencies in a navigation task. Learn Motiv. 2009;40:46–61.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Quaiser-Pohl C, Geiser C, Lehmann W. The relationship between computer-game preference, gender, and mental-rotation ability. Personal Individ Differ. 2006;40(3):609–19.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Terlecki M, Brown J, Harner-Steciw L, Irvin-Hannum J, Marchetto-Ryan N, Ruhl L, Wiggins J. Sex differences and similarities in video game experience, preferences, and self-efficacy: implications for the gaming industry. Curr Psychol. 2011;30(1):22–33.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Terlecki MS, Newcombe NS. How important is the digital divide? The relation of computer and videogame usage to gender differences in mental rotation ability. Sex Roles. 2005;53(5–6):433–41.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Burkitt J, Widman D, Saucier DM. Evidence for the influence of testosterone in the performance of spatial navigation in a virtual water maze in women but not in men. Horm Behav. 2007;51(5):649–54.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  30. Driscoll I, Hamilton DA, Yeo RA, Brooks WM, Sutherland RJ. Virtual navigation in humans: the impact of age, sex, and hormones on place learning. Horm Behav. 2005;47(3):326–35.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  31. Hausmann M, Slabbekoorn D, Van Goozen SH, Cohen-Kettenis PT, Güntürkün O. Sex hormones affect spatial abilities during the menstrual cycle. Behav Neurosci. 2000;114(6):1245.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  32. Hausmann M, Schoofs D, Rosenthal HE, Jordan K. Interactive effects of sex hormones and gender stereotypes on cognitive sex differences—a psychobiosocial approach. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2009;34(3):389–401.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  33. Silverman I, Kastuk D, Choi J, Phillips K. Testosterone levels and spatial ability in men. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 1999;24(8):813–22.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  34. Yang CFJ, Hooven CK, Boynes M, Gray PB, Pope HG Jr. Testosterone levels and mental rotation performance in Chinese men. Horm Behav. 2007;51(3):373–8.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  35. McKeever WF, Rich DA, Deyo RA, Conner RL. Androgens and spatial ability: failure to find a relationship between testosterone and ability measures. Bull Psychon Soc. 1987;25(6):438–40.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Puts DA, Cárdenas RA, Bailey DH, Burriss RP, Jordan CL, Breedlove SM. Salivary testosterone does not predict mental rotation performance in men or women. Horm Behav. 2010;58(2):282–9.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  37. Shute VJ, Pellegrino JW, Hubert L, Reynolds RW. The relationship between androgen levels and human spatial abilities. Bull Psychon Soc. 1983;21(6):465–8.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  38. Lawton CA. Gender and regional differences in spatial referents used in direction giving. Sex Roles. 2001;44(5–6):321–37.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  39. Boone AP, Gong X, Hegarty M. Sex differences in navigation strategy and efficiency. Mem Cogn. 2018;46(6):909–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  40. Scheuringer A, Pletzer B. Sex differences and menstrual cycle dependent changes in cognitive strategies during spatial navigation and verbal fluency. Front Psychol. 2017;8:381.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. Lawton CA. Strategies for indoor wayfinding: the role of orientation. J Environ Psychol. 1996;16:137–45.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  42. Eals M, Silverman I. The hunter-gatherer theory of spatial sex differences: proximate factors mediating the female advantage in recall of object arrays. Ethol Sociobiol. 1994;15:95–105.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  43. Silverman, I., and Eals, M. Sex differences in spatial abilities: evolutionary theory and data. In The 98 J. M. Dabbs et al. Adapted mind: evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture, J.H. Barlow, L. Cosmides, and J. Tooby (Eds.). New York: Oxford, 1992, pp. 541–549.

  44. Denis M. The description of routes: a cognitive approach to the production of spatial discourse. Cahiers de Psychologie Cognitive/Current Psychology of Cognition. 1997;16:409–58.

    Google Scholar 

  45. Miller LK, Santoni V. Sex differences in spatial abilities: strategic and experiential correlates. Acta Psychol. 1986;62(3):225–35.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  46. Montello DR, Richardson AE, Hegarty M, Provenza M. A comparison of methods for estimating directions in egocentric space. Perception. 1999;28(8):981–1000.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  47. Lawton CA, Kallai J. Gender differences in wayfinding strategies and anxiety about wayfinding: a cross-cultural comparison. Sex Roles. 2002;47(9–10):389–401.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  48. Sandstrom NJ, Kaufman J, Huettel SA. Males and females use different distal cues in a virtual environment navigation task. Cogn Brain Res. 1998;6(4):351–60.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  49. Fehring RJ, Schneider M, Raviele K. Variability in the phases of the menstrual cycle. J Obstet Gynecol Neonatal Nurs. 2006;35(3):376–84.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  50. Hussain D, Hanafi S, Konishi K, Brake WG, Bohbot VD. Modulation of spatial and response strategies by phase of the menstrual cycle in women tested in a virtual navigation task. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2016;70:108–17.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  51. Raven JC, Court JH. Raven’s progressive matrices and vocabulary scales. Oxford: Oxford Psychologists Press Ltd; 1998.

  52. Wiener Testsystem. Schuhfried GmbH. (1995); Gaschok, J. (2002). Wiener Testsystem. In U.P. Kanning & H. Holling (Hrsg.), Handbuch personaldiagnostischer Instrumente (S. 485–491). Göttingen: Hogrefe.

  53. Bates D, Maechler M, Bolker B, Walker S. Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. J Stat Softw. 2015;67(1):1 48. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v067.i01 .

    Article  Google Scholar 

  54. Verde P, Piccardi L, Bianchini F, Trivelloni P, Guariglia C, Tomao E. Gender effects on mental rotation in pilots vs. nonpilots. Aviat Space Environ Med. 2013;84(7):726–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  55. Verde P, Piccardi L, Bianchini F, Guariglia C, Carrozzo P, Morgagni F, Tomao E. Gender differences in navigational memory: pilots vs. nonpilots. Aerosp Med Hum Perform. 2015;86(2):103–11.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  56. Verde P, Boccia M, Colangeli S, Barbetti S, Nori R, Ferlazzo F, Piccardi L. Domain-specific interference tests on navigational working memory in military pilots. Aerosp Med Hum Perform. 2016;87(6):528–33.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  57. Byrne P, Becker S, Burgess N. Remembering the past and imagining the future: a neural model of spatial memory and imagery. Psychol Rev. 2007;114(2):340.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  58. Foreman NP, Arber M, Savage J. Spatial memory in preschool infants. Dev Psychobiol. 1984;17(2):129–37.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  59. Foreman N, Warry R, Murray P. Development of reference and working spatial memory in preschool children. J Gen Psychol. 1990;117(3):267.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank the students of Belinda Pletzer for their help with participant recruitment. Also, we would like to thank all participants for their willingness to participate in this study.

Funding

This research was supported by the Austrian science fund (FWF - P28261).

Availability of data and materials

Please contact the author for data requests.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

TH and BP wrote the main manuscript text. TH prepared the figures. AS wrote parts of the “Introduction” section. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to TiAnni Harris.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

All participants gave their written consent to participate in the study. The methods used conformed to the Code of Ethics of the World Medical Association (Declaration of Helsinki).

The institutional guidelines of the University of Salzburg (Statutes of the University of Salzburg1) state in §163 (1) that it is necessary to seek ethical approval for research on human subjects if the physical or psychological integrity is affected, the right for privacy or other important rights or interests of the subjects or their dependents are confounded. Paragraph §163 (2) states that it is the decision and responsibility of the PI to decide, whether (1) applies to a study or not. Therefore, no ethical approval for this study was sought out. Non-invasive methods were used on healthy adult volunteers, who willingly gave their informed consent to participate. Accordingly, (1) did not apply. Data were processed in anonymized/de-identified form. Participants were assigned a subject ID (v001, v002, etc.), when physically present at the lab, which was used throughout the study.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Harris, T., Scheuringer, A. & Pletzer, B. Perspective and strategy interactively modulate sex differences in a 3D navigation task. Biol Sex Differ 10, 17 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1186/s13293-019-0232-z

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s13293-019-0232-z